Not all memories fade with age

August 22, 2013 by David Pacchioli
Nancy Dennis, assistant professor of psychology at Penn State, studies memory and aging. Credit: Patrick Mansell

It may happen more than you care to admit. You draw on a memory from the not-so-distant past, and the bucket comes back empty. Your 10-year-old child, however, can recall the moment in a flash. "It was last summer. That street-fair we went to in Delaware. The guy with the wrinkly dog."

The details then flood back—but without the youthful prompt, they were lost in the mist. Do you just chalk it up to old age?

"Everybody thinks: 'Memory declines with age,'" says cognitive neuroscientist Nancy Dennis. "And it does, to some degree. But it isn't that simple. Some types of memory fade as we age, but others are very stable."

Dennis, an assistant professor of psychology at Penn State, studies the . Using functional MRI, which offers a look at in real-time, she identifies changes that occur as people get older, then tries to match this neural evidence with . "The one thing memory researchers have found," she says, "is that as we age, the brain activates different regions for certain than it does in younger people. The big question is 'Why?' What do these changes in brain activity mean?"

Memory, she explains, is not a single construct. What know as "item memory," the ability to recall individual events or impressions, is actually quite stable in , she says. What tends to fall off is "association memory," the ability to link those impressions. You might remember a name or recognize a face, in other words, but putting the two together becomes a challenge.

In a similar way, older adults typically hold their own in "familiarity," remembering the gist of an event or conversation, but do less well at "recollection," calling up specific details.

Association memory and recollection are both governed by the , a region in the medial temporal lobe, Dennis says. Of all the brain's regions, she notes, the hippocampus shows some of the greatest loss of volume associated with aging. By contrast, the area surrounding the hippocampus shows much less shrinkage. "It's believed that these other structures are more responsible for familiarity and item memory," she says.

Indeed, among older adults asked to complete memory tasks while their brain activity is being monitored by the fMRI scanner, Dennis sees less activity in the hippocampus and more in the surrounding regions—the parahippocampal gyrus and the entorhinal cortex—when compared to younger subjects.

Nancy Dennis, left, assists a subject while scanner technician Amanda Gearhart connects a 12-channel head coil in place over the subject's head to prepare for an MRI scan in the Social, Life, and Engineering Sciences Imaging Center located in Chandlee Laboratory on Penn State's University Park campus. By combining functional MRI scans with tests of memory performance, Nancy Dennis and her associates are gaining a better understanding of age-related memory loss. Credit: Patrick Mansell

Getting the gist

But what exactly does the shift in activity mean? Is the diminishing brain simply throwing out all lifelines, casting about desperately for assistance from any quarter? Or is the brain adroitly rewiring itself, adapting to one region's diminishment by arranging for other, stronger regions to take over certain functions?

Ten years ago, Roberto Cabeza, a neuroscientist at Duke University who advised Dennis when she was a postdoctoral fellow there, published an influential paper advancing the compensatory view. Imaging the brains of young adults, older adults who did well on memory tests, and older adults who did poorly on the tests, Cabeza showed that the first and last groups were using regions on only one side of the brain to complete the tasks, while the middle group—what he called "high-performing" older adults—were calling on both sides of the brain. The high-performers' brains, he concluded, had successfully adapted to age-related neural decline.

The shift in activity, Dennis says, reflects a successful change in cognitive strategy, as older adults tend to rely more on getting the gist of an experience than younger adults do, and less on grasping the details. But adaptation has its limits, she notes. The reliance on familiarity runs into trouble where two or more experiences are basically similar. "When memories share a common gist they can easily get confused," she says. This confusion can lead to what's known as false memory.

"When we think of memory problems, we tend to think only of forgetting," Dennis explains. "False memory is remembering things that never happened. You could swear you took the dog out last night, or swallowed this morning's pill, but you didn't."

In this case, the brain tends to fill in the specifics based on with past events. In one current study, Dennis notes, she shows photos of typical domestic scenes—kitchens and bathrooms—then asks subjects to recall their details. Was there a sink in that kitchen? "People tend to answer yes," she says, even if there is no sink present, "because their mind knows a kitchen is supposed to have a sink. It tries to fill in the blanks."

This tendency can have more serious implications in matters such as eyewitness identification. "There's a lot of research that shows that in the absence of the actual person in a line-up, people tend to endorse the person that most closely resembles the actual person," Dennis says. "They're relying on gist. The problem is that now you have a true memory of the wrong person, and your mind tends to build on that."

Here again, brain scans can offer insight, Dennis says. In younger adults monitored by fMRI, there is very little change in terms of brain activity between false memory and true memory. "Aside from a few select regions, such as the hippocampus, neurally, you almost can't tell the difference," she says. In older adults, however, the brain regions activated for false memories are different than those recruited for true memories, suggesting a different cognitive process.

A four-button response box allows the research participant to answer questions aimed at gauging memory performance while the participant is in the MRI scanner. Credit: Patrick Mansell

Trying to forget

One key to help in accurate remembering is the ability to forget what's inessential. Intentional forgetting, Dennis says, is an exercise of cognitive control. "It can be a very beneficial strategy. You forget where you parked your car yesterday so you can remember where you parked it today." This ability to tune out irrelevant information has also been shown to decline with age.

Even when older adults can intentionally forget, Dennis finds, the brain regions responsible are not the same ones that younger adults employ. "On the scans you can see that younger adults are using the regions associated with inhibition and suppression, while older adults are using those associated with attention and focus," she says. "It may be that older adults are switching their attention away while are suppressing the encoding of information."

A certain amount of age-related memory loss is to be expected, she says. In the case of normal decline, however, behavioral strategies may help. Dennis hopes soon to start work with a colleague at another university who is trying out different approaches to help older adults with association memory. Her role, she says, will be to assess the corresponding neural activity, helping to determine which strategies are most beneficial and why.

An MRI scan of a research subject's brain is displayed on a computer at Penn State's 3T MRI facility. By combining MRI scans with behavioral memory tasks, the research team continues to learn more and more about age-related memory loss. Credit: Patrick Mansell

"Different strategies may work better for different tasks," she says. "But whether is stable or shows decline, there are strategies we can use to improve on performance."

That comes as welcome news. After all, you can't always rely on your kids to remember.

Explore further: How we create false memories: Assessing memory performance in older adults

Related Stories

Scientists make older adults less forgetful in memory tests

February 21, 2013

Scientists at Baycrest Health Sciences' Rotman Research Institute (RRI) and the University of Toronto's Psychology Department have found compelling evidence that older adults can eliminate forgetfulness and perform as well ...

Exercise may be the best medicine for Alzheimer's disease

July 30, 2013

New research out of the University of Maryland School of Public Health shows that exercise may improve cognitive function in those at risk for Alzheimer's by increasing the efficiency of brain activity associated with memory. ...

Young vs. old: Who performs more consistently?

August 6, 2013

Sometimes it's just not your day: First you can't remember where you put your car keys, then you forget about an important meeting at work. On days like that, our memory seems to let us down. But are there actually "good" ...

Recommended for you

Umbilical cells help eye's neurons connect

November 24, 2015

Cells isolated from human umbilical cord tissue have been shown to produce molecules that help retinal neurons from the eyes of rats grow, connect and survive, according to Duke University researchers working with Janssen ...

Brain connections predict how well you can pay attention

November 24, 2015

During a 1959 television appearance, Jack Kerouac was asked how long it took him to write his novel On The Road. His response – three weeks – amazed the interviewer and ignited an enduring myth that the book was composed ...

No cable spaghetti in the brain

November 24, 2015

Our brain is a mysterious machine. Billions of nerve cells are connected such that they store information as efficiently as books are stored in a well-organized library. To this date, many details remain unclear, for instance ...

Neurons encoding hand shapes identified in human brain

November 23, 2015

Neural prosthetic devices, which include small electrode arrays implanted in the brain, can allow paralyzed patients to control the movement of a robotic limb, whether that limb is attached to the individual or not. In May ...

Wireless sensor enables study of traumatic brain injury

November 23, 2015

A new system that uses a wireless implant has been shown to record for the first time how brain tissue deforms when subjected to the kind of shock that causes blast-induced trauma commonly seen in combat veterans.

1 comment

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

not rated yet Aug 23, 2013
History can not be repeated with total recall.
I hold the acting profession against your speculations reported here:

Are the roles the acting profession portrays false memories?

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.