Neuroscientists suggest perception of harmonicity, not beating underlies perception of dissonance

November 13, 2012 by Bob Yirka, Medical Xpress report
Steinway & Sons Grand Piano Iron Plates and Strings. Credit: David Maiolo / Wikipedia.

(Medical Xpress)—Researchers from the University of Montreal and New York University suggest in a paper they've had published in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, that the perception of harmonicity in humans is more to blame for a dislike of dissonance in musical tones than is beating.

For many years scientists have debated the issue of whether the dislike of dissonant tones by most people in western cultures is due to a , or a simple preference for more harmonically pleasing sounds. Those arguing for the former suggest that the rattling noise that occurs when two tones are close in frequency (beating), which results in roughness in the ears, is naturally aversive and thus the answer is that it's a physical condition. Those that argue for the second point out that there are incidences in music, such as rock, where people have learned to enjoy roughness, and thus an aversion to dissonant tones is more likely the result of something in that are preferred.

In this new research, the team dug a little deeper by enlisting volunteers that suffer from amusia – a condition whereby those who have it have perceive tones differently than most people. They ran several tests comparing the perception of tones in people with amusia with those of a control group that had normal hearing.

In the first test, they asked both groups to rate the pleasantness of a series of intervals (notes at predefined distances between frequencies). The group with amusia rated them all equally, whereas the group with normal hearing rated some intervals as much more pleasant or unpleasant than others.

They followed up the first test by simply asking both groups how they felt about beating – whether they were able to detect it and whether it bothered them. Both groups reported being able to hear it and to have an aversion to it. This, the researchers suggest, indicates that there is some other property at work in causing to report aversion to .

They suggest that rather than finding dissonance aversive, it's more that people find other tones more pleasant and they claim that it's because of the harmonicity of constant intervals. For pleasant or consonant intervals, they say the overtones that are produced as tones are played coincide rather than collide, producing what most perceive as something pleasant.

Turning back to the volunteers, the researchers found that the control group did indeed report finding consonant intervals with harmonic relationships preferable to those deemed inharmonic. Those with amusia on the other hand, reported no differences in preference. The researchers conclude that their results throw doubt on the idea that an aversion to certain tones when played together is the result of a physical attribute of the human ear or the way they are interpreted in the brain.

Explore further: Scientists show how adversity dulls our perceptions

More information: The basis of musical consonance as revealed by congenital amusia, PNAS, Published online before print November 12, 2012, doi: 10.1073/pnas.1207989109

Abstract
Some combinations of musical notes sound pleasing and are termed "consonant," but others sound unpleasant and are termed "dissonant." The distinction between consonance and dissonance plays a central role in Western music, and its origins have posed one of the oldest and most debated problems in perception. In modern times, dissonance has been widely believed to be the product of "beating": interference between frequency components in the cochlea that has been believed to be more pronounced in dissonant than consonant sounds. However, harmonic frequency relations, a higher-order sound attribute closely related to pitch perception, has also been proposed to account for consonance. To tease apart theories of musical consonance, we tested sound preferences in individuals with congenital amusia, a neurogenetic disorder characterized by abnormal pitch perception. We assessed amusics' preferences for musical chords as well as for the isolated acoustic properties of beating and harmonicity. In contrast to control subjects, amusic listeners showed no preference for consonance, rating the pleasantness of consonant chords no higher than that of dissonant chords. Amusics also failed to exhibit the normally observed preference for harmonic over inharmonic tones, nor could they discriminate such tones from each other. Despite these abnormalities, amusics exhibited normal preferences and discrimination for stimuli with and without beating. This dissociation indicates that, contrary to classic theories, beating is unlikely to underlie consonance. Our results instead suggest the need to integrate harmonicity as a foundation of music preferences, and illustrate how amusia may be used to investigate normal auditory function.

Related Stories

Scientists show how adversity dulls our perceptions

May 11, 2011
Adversity, we are told, heightens our senses, imprinting sights and sounds precisely in our memories. But new Weizmann Institute research, which appeared in Nature Neuroscience this week, suggests the exact opposite may be ...

Researchers find head turning resets cocktail party effect

April 10, 2012
(Medical Xpress) -- Anyone who has ever been to a cocktail party knows how difficult it can be to hear and follow conversations due to a host of distracting noises. Some might have even noticed that if they focus on a person ...

What makes music sound so sweet (or not)

May 20, 2010
Ever since ancient times, scholars have puzzled over the reasons that some musical note combinations sound so sweet while others are just downright dreadful. The Greeks believed that simple ratios in the string lengths of ...

Lesson in sleep learning: Associations formed in brains of sleeping volunteers remained intact when subjects were awake

August 26, 2012
Is sleep learning possible? A new Weizmann Institute study appearing today in Nature Neuroscience has found that if certain odors are presented after tones during sleep, people will start sniffing when they hear the tones ...

Research duo uncovers clues about why 'fingernails across a chalkboard' is so horrible

November 3, 2011
(Medical Xpress) -- Proving that science isn’t always just fun and games, two researchers with music backgrounds have conducted experiments to get to the bottom of why people are so adversely effected by certain noises ...

Researchers devise a way to manipulate a rat's dreams

September 6, 2012
(Medical Xpress)—Cognitive scientists working at MIT have devised a means for not only altering the dreams of rats, but of demonstrating a way of testing what they've achieved, offering evidence that it can be done, and ...

Recommended for you

Are you prone to feeling guilty? Then you're probably more trustworthy, study shows

July 19, 2018
It turns out your mother was right: guilt is a powerful motivator.

Perfectionism in young children may indicate OCD risk

July 19, 2018
Studying young children, researchers at Washington University School of Medicine in St. Louis found that kids who possess tendencies toward perfectionism and excessive self-control are twice as likely as other children to ...

Finding well-being through an aerial, as opposed to ground-level, view of time

July 19, 2018
Do today and yesterday and tomorrow loom large in your thinking, with the more distant past and future barely visible on the horizon? That's not unusual in today's time-pressed world—and it seems a recipe for angst.

Younger children tend to make more informed decisions

July 19, 2018
A new study from the University of Waterloo has found that in some ways, the older you get the worse your decision making becomes.

Using an electronic device counteracts benefits of taking a break in nature, researchers find

July 19, 2018
Being in nature helps restore your brain's ability to focus attention on a task. But if you are checking social media on your phone or answering emails on your laptop – even if you are doing so while surrounded by trees ...

Depression-induced inflammation during pregnancy may impact newborns

July 18, 2018
The physiological impacts of depression on pregnant mothers may affect babies while in the womb and lead to changes in the behaviour and biology of newborns, finds new King's College London research.

1 comment

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

MrVibrating
not rated yet Nov 13, 2012
The issue here is akin to the the 18th century confusion over the nature of temperature, and the notion that perhaps heat and coldness were distinct substances.

Similarly, the key to understanding dissonance is to note that maximum consonance is equivalence (frequencies in a factor of two relationship to a given fundamental).

Thus all shades of consonance and dissonance are but degrees of equivalence. In a nutshell, dissonance is inequivalence.

The octave is equivalent, the tritone is not-at-all equivalent.

The increments along the spectrum are the number of cycles of the lower tone to which those of the upper resolve - so for example octaves resolve every other cycle, but tritones, every fifth cycle.

The size of the temporal integration window required to process harmonic intervals thus increases as they become more 'dissonant', and decreases to a minimum size at maximum consonance (ie. equivalence).

Ergo, dissonance is inefficient to process - it is more cortically demanding.

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.