Psychology researchers extend knowledge of visual misperception

September 13, 2013 by Jessica Luton, University of Georgia

Using abstract images instead of real photographs, University of Georgia researchers are one step closer to understanding visual misperceptions and discovering just why people experience a phenomenon known as boundary extension.

Boundary extension happens when someone takes a look at a scene, glances away and remembers seeing a more wide-angle view than was actually present. In just a few short seconds, the helps most people extend the scene beyond what is actually seen.

Scientists at the University of Delaware discovered this concept in 1989 when they showed study participants real photographs of 20 scenes for 15 seconds and then had participants draw a picture of what they'd viewed. Ninety-five percent of the drawings included information that wasn't physically present but would have been just outside the camera's field of view.

This phenomenon has been consistently demonstrated since that time, but researchers have not been able to explain why it occurs.

James Brown, an associate professor in the Franklin College of Arts and Sciences department of psychology, and graduate students Benjamin McDunn and Aisha Siddiqui decided to study whether the use of an abstract image—instead of a photograph—would produce different results.

They found that showing subjects geometrical images on random-dot or white backgrounds had similar results. The study was published recently in the journal Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

"This study is consistent with many areas of perception, demonstrating the natural tendency for our perceptual systems to go beyond, fill in and extrapolate from the current sensory input," Brown said. "Here we show this tendency as it relates to scene perception. Even with completely novel 'scenes' where we have no experience with or knowledge about them, we still tend to extrapolate beyond what was originally seen when our memory is tested soon after viewing them."

Familiarity with an environment or an object may have much less of an effect than was previously thought on whether or not people remember viewing things through a wide-angle lens.

"The most valuable finding is support for the idea that boundary extension is likely happening constantly during our perception of close-up views of the world, regardless of context," he said. "We showed evidence that people may be automatically priming themselves with expectations of what lies just outside their current view even with images depicting an unrealistic and abstract setting. This suggests that our familiarity with an environment doesn't have as much effect on whether or not extension occurs as current theory would have expected."

McDunn, who completed his master's degree in psychology earlier this year, is now working on his doctorate and continuing his study of this concept.

"Subtracting out different pieces of the puzzle—conceptual knowledge of view and object recognition—has helped tease out key characteristics of our ability to make sense of the world visually," he said.

For Brown and his team, "the real challenge has been finding different kinds of stimuli that don't elicit ," he said. "Doing so will help establish the parameters of the effect and, hopefully, lead to a better understanding of its causes."

McDunn and Brown are now examining spatial perception by constructing a study in which participants view scenes depicting the real world—with similar object features such as size and spatial attributes—to see if the phenomenon exhibits with the same pattern.

"Spatial vision in general is somewhat enigmatic and difficult to study in isolation," McDunn said. "Boundary extension may provide a means of determining what characteristics of a stimulus cause us to perceive it as depicting space."

While researching spatial vision is difficult, Brown said, it's important to understand more about how people see the world.

"Boundary extension," he said, "seems to be strongly tied to our perception of a continuous spatial expanse around us, knowing a given view depicts a truncated portion of the world, a characteristic that is common to all scenes in the real world."

Explore further: Research explores common visual error of 'boundary extension'

More information: www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23921509

Related Stories

Research explores common visual error of 'boundary extension'

March 9, 2012
(Medical Xpress) -- Helene Intraub, professor of psychology at the University of Delaware, and then-undergraduate researcher Mike Richardson first published their paper on the phenomenon of "boundary extension" in 1989, describing ...

Study sheds new light on a classic question in psychology and neuroscience

June 27, 2013
A study from Karolinska Institutet in Sweden shows, that our imagination may affect how we experience the world more than we perhaps think. What we imagine hearing or seeing "in our head" can change our actual perception. ...

World-first research to explain why actions speak louder than words

August 9, 2013
An innovative series of experiments could help to unlock the mysteries of how the brain makes sense of the hustle and bustle of human activity we see around us every day.

Language can reveal the invisible, study shows

August 26, 2013
It is natural to imagine that the sense of sight takes in the world as it is—simply passing on what the eyes collect from light reflected by the objects around us.

Recommended for you

People with prosthetic arms less affected by common illusion

January 22, 2018
People with prosthetic arms or hands do not experience the "size-weight illusion" as strongly as other people, new research shows.

Study of learning and memory problems in OCD helps young people unlock potential at school

January 22, 2018
Adolescents with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) have widespread learning and memory problems, according to research published today. The findings have already been used to assist adolescents with OCD obtain the help ...

Intensive behavior therapy no better than conventional support in treating teenagers with antisocial behavior

January 19, 2018
Research led by UCL has found that intensive and costly multisystemic therapy is no better than conventional therapy in treating teenagers with moderate to severe antisocial behaviour.

Babies' babbling betters brains, language

January 18, 2018
Babies are adept at getting what they need - including an education. New research shows that babies organize mothers' verbal responses, which promotes more effective language instruction, and infant babbling is the key.

College branding makes beer more salient to underage students

January 18, 2018
In recent years, major beer companies have tried to capitalize on the salience of students' university affiliations, unveiling marketing campaigns and products—such as "fan cans," store displays, and billboard ads—that ...

Inherited IQ can increase in early childhood

January 18, 2018
When it comes to intelligence, environment and education matter – more than we think.

1 comment

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

beleg
1 / 5 (1) Sep 14, 2013
If the participants were given the statement:
"Draw EXCATELY what you see - nothing more, nothing less"
the research becomes credible.

The participants never received such a statement.

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.