'Sensitive' older sibling may help boost preschoolers' language skills

January 27, 2014 by Amy Norton, Healthday Reporter
'Sensitive' older sibling may help boost preschoolers' language skills
Study found a difference, particularly in large families where parents are stretched for time.

(HealthDay)—Preschoolers with lots of brothers and sisters seem to develop language skills a bit slower than other kids—possibly because they get less attention from mom and dad.

But a new study suggests that a "sensitive" older sibling might make up for it.

A number of studies have found that with a brood of kids have less time to dedicate to any one 's learning. And some have shown that the younger children in those families tend to have less-developed vocabularies compared to their first-born sibling.

In the new study of 385 preschoolers, researchers found that those from bigger families (at least three kids) did fare a bit worse on a standard test. But that gap was erased if a child had an attentive older sibling.

Experts say the findings, reported online Jan. 27 and in the February print issue of Pediatrics, show that it's not only parents who help young children learn.

In studies on child development, "there's a big focus on parents," noted lead researcher Heather Prime, a doctoral student in at the University of Toronto.

"But there is a place for sibling influences as well," Prime said.

Based on her team's findings, it's a "cognitively sensitive" older sibling who makes a difference. That basically means the older child is in tune with what a young sibling can understand, and changes the way he or she talks to match that—maybe slowing down or using simpler sentences, but not "baby-talking" them, either.

"They pick up on cues the younger sibling is giving them," Prime said. "They might notice they're struggling with something, for example, and offer them extra help."

The findings are based on 385 Ontario families who were part of a larger, long-term study on . Researchers followed one child from each family starting at birth.

When those children were 3 years old, the researchers had them play a picture game that measures "receptive" vocabulary—the words a child understands when other people say them. They also observed the children as they played a structured game with their older sibling, who was about 5 years old, on average.

Overall, the study found, preschoolers from big families tended to score a bit lower on the vocabulary test. And the relationship was still apparent when the researchers weighed other factors—such as family income, and whether the parents were foreign-born or spoke a language other than English at home.

However, in cases where those kids had a sensitive big brother or sister, the vocabulary disadvantage disappeared.

And it wasn't just because parents were doing something right, because the researchers also measured mothers' cognitive sensitivity in interactions with their children.

"Even controlling for what moms were doing, we still saw the importance of the sibling," Prime said.

"This is a very interesting study," said Diane Paul, director of clinical issues in speech-language pathology for the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association in Rockville, Md. "And the findings make a lot of sense. We know that learning language is a social process."

What's "remarkable," Paul said, is that even kindergarten-age children are able to perceive the needs of a younger sibling and change how they communicate. "It seems like they're helping to create a language-learning environment," she added.

Paul noted that, according to one theory, an older sibling might get in the way of a preschoolers' —by speaking for the younger child, for example. And that might be true in some cases.

But based on these findings, Paul said, a sensitive older sibling may be an asset. She added, though, that this study only looked at one aspect of language development—receptive vocabulary.

"It would be nice to study other measures, too, like expressive vocabulary," Paul said. Expressive vocabulary refers to the words not only understand, but use.

And how does a kindergartner become cognitively sensitive to their younger sibling? It's not clear, Prime said. It may be a combination of nature and what they learn from their parents and other people in their lives.

But both Prime and Paul said parents can do things to encourage more-sensitive interactions between their young kids.

An example Prime offered: Five-year-old Johnny and 3-year-old Susie are fighting over a toy. Instead of just saying, "Susie had it first," the parents could encourage Johnny to think about how Susie would feel having that toy taken away from her.

Saying "your sister had it first" may teach a child a rule, Prime noted. But the other approach helps them learn to consider their younger sibling's thoughts and feelings.

Explore further: Sibling bullying: What's the big deal?

More information: The American Speech-Language-Hearing Association has more on child language development.

Related Stories

Sibling bullying: What's the big deal?

September 23, 2013
Sibling bullying is a type of violence that is prevalent in the lives of most children, but little is known about it, researchers say.

Mothers see their youngest as shorter than they are

December 16, 2013
Many parents say when their second child is born that their first child suddenly appears to have grown overnight. Now, researchers reporting in the Cell Press journal Current Biology on December 16 have an explanation: until ...

Babbling babies—responding to one-on-one 'baby talk'—master more words

January 6, 2014
Common advice to new parents is that the more words babies hear the faster their vocabulary grows. Now new findings show that what spurs early language development isn't so much the quantity of words as the style of speech ...

Growing up bilingual: Dual-language upbringing reflected in young children's vocabulary

January 16, 2013
Language mixing – using elements from two languages in the same sentence – is frequent among bilingual parents and could pose a challenge for vocabulary acquisition by one- and two-year-old children, according to a new ...

Talking directly to toddlers strengthens their language skills

October 22, 2013
(Medical Xpress)—Just as young children need nourishing food to build physical strength, they also need linguistic nutrition for optimal development of language and cognitive abilities.

Reading wordless storybooks to toddlers may expose them to richer language, study finds

April 18, 2013
(Medical Xpress)—Researchers at the University of Waterloo have found that children hear more complex language from parents when they read a storybook with only pictures compared to a picture-vocabulary book. The findings ...

Recommended for you

Small drop in measles vaccinations would have outsized effect, study estimates

July 24, 2017
Small reductions in childhood measles vaccinations in the United States would produce disproportionately large increases in the number of measles cases and in related public health costs, according to a new study by researchers ...

At the cellular level, a child's loss of a father is associated with increased stress

July 18, 2017
The absence of a father—due to incarceration, death, separation or divorce—has adverse physical and behavioral consequences for a growing child. But little is known about the biological processes that underlie this link ...

New comparison chart sheds light on babies' tears

July 10, 2017
A chart that enables parents and clinicians to calculate if a baby is crying more than it should in the first three months of its life has been created by a Kingston University London researcher, following a study of colic ...

Blood of SIDS infants contains high levels of serotonin

July 3, 2017
Blood samples from infants who died of Sudden Infant Death Syndrome (SIDS) had high levels of serotonin, a chemical that carries signals along and between nerves, according to a study funded in part by the National Institutes ...

Is your child's 'penicillin allergy' real?

July 3, 2017
(HealthDay)—Many children suspected of being allergic to the inexpensive, first-line antibiotic penicillin actually aren't, new research indicates.

Probiotic supplements failed to prevent babies' infections

July 3, 2017
(HealthDay)—Probiotic supplements may not protect babies from catching colds or stomach bugs in day care, a new clinical trial suggests.

0 comments

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.