New study reveals that prelinguistic infants can categorize colors

February 10, 2016
New study reveals that prelinguistic infants can categorize colors
(Top) Infants watched figures whose colors alternate. NIRS probe set is fitted on an infant's head with bands. (Bottom) Sequence of color changes in the experiment. Colors alternated every 1s. B1 represents a color of blue category and G1/G2 represent two different colors of green category. Color differences between G1-B1, and G1-G2 are equated. Credit: Ichiro Kuriki

A joint group of researchers from Chuo University, Japan Women's University and Tohoku University has revealed that infants aged between 5 and 7 months hold the representation of color categories in their brain, even before the acquisition of language.

This study is published in the of Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

A long-held theory called Sapir-Wharf hypothesis claims that languages define our perceptions. This theory is widely accepted in various fields of study including psychology, linguistics and anthropology. Color perception is also considered to be subject to this theory, since colors are called by their names in daily communications.

Through numerous studies on the color lexicons of languages in the world, categorical color perception is considered to be strongly affected by language. On the other hand, the similarity of color categories across linguistic and cultural differences is also reported as strong evidence of the universality of color categories. Therefore, whether or not language affects color categories has been a central issue related to how we perceive colors.

This new study reveals that the category of colors can be independent of language, at least in the of development in an infant's visual system.

New study reveals that prelinguistic infants can categorize colors
Time course of brain activity changes in infants' occipito-temporal (OT) region. Vertical axis indicates normalized concentration of oxy-hemoglobin (oxy-Hb) in Z-score. Red and blue lines indicate responses to color alternations between blue and green categories, and between different shades of green, respectively. Dashed colored lines indicate +/- 1 standard errors of mean. Vertical dotted lines at time 0s and 10s indicate onset and offset of visual stimulus, respectively. Credit: Ichiro Kuriki

Infants 5-7 months old were tested to see if brain activity is different for colors in different categories. The brain activity was measured by a near infrared specrtoscopy (NIRS; Figure 1) technique, which realizes comfortable measurement of brain activity in infants.

The study found that the brain activity increased significantly when the colors of blue and green were alternated, while there was no significant reaction to the alternation of different shades of green (Figure 2). The difference was observed in the occipito-temporal area in both left and right hemispheres.

A similar difference was found in adult participants with no significant lateralization. Since language related cortical areas reside in the in most right-handed adults, the observed brain activity had no direct relation to language processing. In addition, brain activity caused by categorical color differences was not found in the occipital region, which is known to play a significant role in the early stage of visual processing.

New study reveals that prelinguistic infants can categorize colors
Time course of brain activity in adults' OT regions is shown. Same visual stimuli as the infants' experiment were used. Symbols are the same as in Figure 2. Credit: Ichiro Kuriki

These results show that color information is processed through multiple cortical stages in infants, in a way similar to adults. They suggest that the in reaction to different color categories are represented differently in infants, even before the acquisition of language. They also imply that color categories can develop independent of the acquisition of relevant .

Explore further: When the color we see isn't the color we remember

More information: Jiale Yang et al. Cortical response to categorical color perception in infants investigated by near-infrared spectroscopy, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (2016). DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1512044113

Related Stories

When the color we see isn't the color we remember

June 2, 2015

Though people can distinguish among millions of colors, we have trouble remembering specific shades because our brains tend to store what we've seen as one of just a few basic hues, a Johns Hopkins University-led team discovered.

Subliminal effect of facial color on fearful faces

October 22, 2015

Facial color is suggestive of emotional states, as in the phrases: "flushed with anger" and "pale with fear." Although some behavioral studies have investigated the effects of facial color on expression, there is limited ...

How does color blindness affect color preferences?

July 21, 2015

(Medical Xpress)—Dichromacy is a color vision defect in which one of the three types of cone photoreceptors is missing. The condition is hereditary and sex-linked, mostly affecting males. Although researchers have explored ...

Recommended for you

Video games can change your brain

June 22, 2017

Scientists have collected and summarized studies looking at how video games can shape our brains and behavior. Research to date suggests that playing video games can change the brain regions responsible for attention and ...

Researchers discover brain inflammation in people with OCD

June 21, 2017

A new brain imaging study by the Centre for Addiction and Mental Health (CAMH) shows for the first time that brain inflammation is significantly elevated - more than 30 per cent higher - in people with obsessive-compulsive ...

0 comments

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.