How brain maturation changes uninhibited teens to self-controlled adults

March 16, 2016 by Christopher Packham, Medical Xpress report
brain
White matter fiber architecture of the brain. Credit: Human Connectome Project.

(Medical Xpress)—One of the biggest problems in the U.S. justice system is the expanding number of states that treat teens as adults for certain crimes. While harsh treatment of adolescents might generate votes for tough-on-crime policies and candidates, an increasing amount of neurological research strongly suggests that the brain function of teens is vastly different than adult brains, and that impulsivity in adolescents is largely unavoidable.

However, little is yet known about the physiological development of prefrontal neurons after puberty. In the past, it's been established that the ability to resist impulsive responses matures after puberty, but neurologists want to know how this maps to changes in the brain. Recently, a collaborative of U.S. researchers published the results of a longitudinal study in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences that tracked the neuronal responses of monkeys as they transitioned from puberty to adulthood, comparing brain activity at different stages of development.

Puberty begins in monkeys around 3.5 years of age, and they reach sexual maturity at around five years old. This corresponds to the human ages of approximately 11 years and 16 years, respectively. The researchers made neurophysiological recordings with a group of monkeys trained in an antisaccade task—meaning that they were trained not to look at a particular visual stimulus. They were tested once in and again as adults.

The researchers demonstrate that behavioral response inhibition to the stimulus significantly improved in adulthood. "Performance benefits were observed for all types of errors, including the ability to resist making an eye movement toward the cue," the authors write. But this finding came with one surprising conclusion: "This enhanced control was not achieved through a general slowing of reaction times in the adult stage; to the contrary, adult monkeys needed less time to process the cue and plan a correct saccade."

In the adults, the researchers observed an increase in baseline neural activity, even before the appearance of a cue. This was a significant finding, because low levels of baseline activity—as observed in adolescents—are predictive of errors. The researchers believe that this baseline neural activity is related to the preparation of a response, which is critical to inhibitory control.

The researchers report increased activity of purely visual neurons, and activity associated with vector inversion in the lateral intraparietal area, writing, "It is likely that this change between stages is associated with the neural representation of the goal through processes such as shifting of attention and vector inversion, which correspond to the encoding of a spatial location away from the stimulus." Furthermore, they observed that neurons in the exhibited vector-inversion behavior away from the stimulus without imposing a delay on response time.

The researchers note that their results leave open the possibility that neurophysiological changes may also occur in areas outside the prefrontal cortex that could also contribute to the improved responses of the adult monkeys, but which were not within the scope of the study.

Explore further: Childhood's end: ADHD, autism and schizophrenia tied to stronger inhibitory interactions in adolescent prefrontal cortex

More information: Behavioral response inhibition and maturation of goal representation in prefrontal cortex after puberty. PNAS 2016 ; published ahead of print March 7, 2016, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1518147113

Abstract
Executive functions including behavioral response inhibition mature after puberty, in tandem with structural changes in the prefrontal cortex. Little is known about how activity of prefrontal neurons relates to this profound cognitive development. To examine this, we tracked neuronal responses of the prefrontal cortex in monkeys as they transitioned from puberty into adulthood and compared activity at different developmental stages. Performance of the antisaccade task greatly improved in this period. Among neural mechanisms that could facilitate it, reduction of stimulus-driven activity, increased saccadic activity, or enhanced representation of the opposing goal location, only the latter was evident in adulthood. Greatly accentuated in adults, this neural correlate of vector inversion may be a prerequisite to the formation of a motor plan to look away from the stimulus. Our results suggest that the prefrontal mechanisms that underlie mature performance on the antisaccade task are more strongly associated with forming an alternative plan of action than with suppressing the neural impact of the prepotent stimulus.

Related Stories

Childhood's end: ADHD, autism and schizophrenia tied to stronger inhibitory interactions in adolescent prefrontal cortex

March 14, 2014
(Medical Xpress)—Key cognitive functions such as working memory (which combines temporary storage and manipulation of information) and executive function (a set of mental processes that helps connect past experience with ...

How we keep track of what matters

February 23, 2016
When watching basketball, we are easily able to keep track of the ball while also making frequent eye and head movements to look at the different players. Neuroscientists Tao Yao, Stefan Treue and B. Suresh Krishna from the ...

Waking up the visual system

October 3, 2014
The ways that neurons in the brain respond to a given stimulus depends on whether an organism is asleep, drowsy, awake, paying careful attention or ignoring the stimulus. However, while the properties of neural circuits in ...

Researchers get a look at neural networks supporting working memory

August 21, 2015
(Medical Xpress)—Translating stored information into behaviorally appropriate responses is a pretty fundamental brain activity, supporting biological, social and survival behaviors. Researchers have long believed that the ...

Brain study finds that practice doesn't always make perfect

March 10, 2016
Even our most practiced movements are imperfect. When pro basketball players shoot free throws, they need to release the ball the same way every time. But they still miss game-winning shots.

Study explains why mistakes slow us down, but not necessarily for the better

January 21, 2016
Taking more time to make decisions after a mistake arises from a mixture of adaptive neural mechanisms that improve the accuracy and maladaptive mechanisms that reduce it, neuroscientists at New York University have found. ...

Recommended for you

Cognitive training helps regain a younger-working brain

January 23, 2018
Relentless cognitive decline as we age is worrisome, and it is widely thought to be an unavoidable negative aspect of normal aging. Researchers at the Center for BrainHealth at The University of Texas at Dallas, however, ...

Lifting the veil on 'valence,' brain study reveals roots of desire, dislike

January 23, 2018
The amygdala is a tiny hub of emotions where in 2016 a team led by MIT neuroscientist Kay Tye found specific populations of neurons that assign good or bad feelings, or "valence," to experience. Learning to associate pleasure ...

Your brain responses to music reveal if you're a musician or not

January 23, 2018
How your brain responds to music listening can reveal whether you have received musical training, according to new Nordic research conducted in Finland (University of Jyväskylä and AMI Center) and Denmark (Aarhus University).

New neuron-like cells allow investigation into synthesis of vital cellular components

January 22, 2018
Neuron-like cells created from a readily available cell line have allowed researchers to investigate how the human brain makes a metabolic building block essential for the survival of all living organisms. A team led by researchers ...

Finding unravels nature of cognitive inflexibility in fragile X syndrome

January 22, 2018
Mice with the genetic defect that causes fragile X syndrome (FXS) learn and remember normally, but show an inability to learn new information that contradicts what they initially learned, shows a new study by a team of neuroscientists. ...

Epilepsy linked to brain volume and thickness differences

January 22, 2018
Epilepsy is associated with thickness and volume differences in the grey matter of several brain regions, according to new research led by UCL and the Keck School of Medicine of USC.

1 comment

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

brahmix
1 / 5 (1) Mar 17, 2016
Crime: is it a lack of self control or a failure to adhere to law?

In the world today, where parents are forced to talk to delinquents, can one not expect that the under-developed pre-frontal cortex will not learn the basic cause-and-effect of lawlessness?
This is a manufactured tragedy - California dreaming while the world dies screaming (adlib, ole' Maiden).
Self-control is not a natural eventuality - it is learned. Good culture, good manners, lawfulness - these are products, not of neurons mapping themselves, but by being taught what is Good through good example and consequences to own actions as a very young child- the lessons force the neural path, not the other way around.
So, in short: lack of self control is proof positive of criminality - i.e. a social deviance that is detrimental to society in terms of peace, security and well being.
Frank Hubert, in his wonderful book: 'Dune', linked the lack of self-control to the lack of humanity (Gom Jabbar).

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.