Study reveals how bilinguals use emoticons to find consensus

October 9, 2017, University at Albany
Credit: CC0 Public Domain

Naysayers like to cite the popularity of emoji and emoticons as yet more evidence of the erosion of language and literacy and, perhaps, civilization itself. But studying how people use them can reveal much about our ability to socialize and exchange information.

For example, a recent study by a team of researchers that included UAlbany Professor of Psychology Laurie Feldman includes important findings about how use emoticons to optimize .

"Patterns of emoticon use constitute a novel yet systematic, nonverbal aspect of communication," said Laurie Feldman, one of four researchers on the study. "Adult scientists who do not speak the same first and who worked together for four years altered their use of emoticons and vocabulary depending on the communication style of others in the conversation."

Participating in the study, which was published in July in the journal Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, were 30 astrophysicists in the United States and France. The group collaborated remotely to observe and control a shared telescope.

In an increasingly digitalized world, where cross-cultural collaborations like the one analyzed in the study are bound to become more common, learning ways of bridging communication gaps with new technology becomes ever more valuable.

"Successful communication and decision-making is at the core of productive remote collaboration," the study states. "A key to success is apprehending the experiences and emotional cues of others."

Feldman and her collaborators identified and studied patterns in communication in the AOL Instant Messenger chat logs generated by the scientists. They found that emoticon use among the bilinguals (all French-English speakers) differed in significant ways from their English-speaking counterparts.

The French speakers were fluent in English, so they could have easily expressed themselves in , yet there were differences in the ways they used emoticons relative to the English-only speakers. Bilinguals used both a wider vocabulary of emoticons and a greater number of them per message. More significantly, they were better able to adjust their vocabulary according to their monolingual partners, and this was especially conspicuous for emoticons.

"Emoticons are sensitive to the same cognitive and social pressures as words when we communicate," Feldman said. "It's not a conscious decision, you define a common vocabulary to use and reuse with someone without being aware of it, and it turns out that emoticons are part of that shared vocabulary."

Code switching

When bilinguals "code switch," or alternate between two or more languages, they are changing how they communicate with respect to the words they choose, while adhering to the idea they intend to share.

One reason why translating word-for-word from one language to another is problematic is because some combinations of words take on a meaning that cannot be predicted from the individual words that comprise the message, according to the study.

"In the case of idioms – 'It's raining cats and dogs,' for example—word-to-word translation can even be humorous if words are selected without regard to the overall meaning," Feldman said.

However, the research shows that combining words and emoticons is more like code-blending because emoticons can alter the interpretation of the words that accompany them.

Because emoticons aren't regarded as constituting a true linguistic "code" in and of themselves, "any evidence for trade-offs between words and is particularly interesting," Feldman said.

Feldman, a cognitive psychologist interested in language, joined the UAlbany faculty more than 26 years ago. Her research examines speaking and reading in native speakers and non-native speakers of a language with special attention to the ways we combine meaningful elements (morphemes) to form longer words.

Explore further: Some bilinguals use emoticons more when chatting in non-native language

Related Stories

Some bilinguals use emoticons more when chatting in non-native language

February 17, 2015
Sometimes, a smile can say everything. But has :-)—the emoticon version of a happy grin—crossed that line into becoming a socially acceptable way of communicating?

Why scientists should research emojis and emoticons :-P

January 17, 2017
More than 90% of online populations now incorporate emojis and emoticons into their texts and emails, and researchers are wondering what the use of (~_^), (>_

Most adults know more than 42,000 words

August 16, 2016
How many words do we know? It turns out that even language experts and researchers have a tough time estimating this.

Recommended for you

Even toddlers weigh risks, rewards when making choices

September 21, 2018
Every day, adults conduct cost-benefit analyses in some form for decisions large and small, economic and personal: Bring a lunch or go out? Buy or rent? Remain single or start a family? All are balances of risk and reward.

Early warning sign of psychosis detected

September 21, 2018
Brains of people at risk of psychosis exhibit a pattern that can help predict whether they will go on to develop full-fledged schizophrenia, a new Yale-led study shows. The findings could help doctors begin early intervention ...

Quitting junk food produces similar withdrawal-type symptoms as drug addiction

September 20, 2018
If you plan to try and quit junk food, expect to suffer similar withdrawal-type symptoms—at least during the initial week—like addicts experience when they attempt to quit using drugs.

In depression the brain region for stress control is larger

September 20, 2018
Although depression is one of the leading psychiatric disorders in Germany, its cause remains unclear. A recent study at the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences (MPI CBS) in Leipzig, Germany, found ...

American girls read and write better than boys

September 20, 2018
As early as the fourth grade, girls perform better than boys on standardized tests in reading and writing, and as they get older that achievement gap widens even more, according to research published by the American Psychological ...

Mindfulness meditation: 10 minutes a day improves cognitive function

September 19, 2018
Practising mindfulness meditation for 10 minutes a day improves concentration and the ability to keep information active in one's mind, a function known as "working memory". The brain achieves this by becoming more efficient, ...

0 comments

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.