Researchers identify neural mechanisms that strengthen or weaken memories during sleep

October 11, 2017, University of Barcelona
Researchers identify neural mechanisms that strengthen or weaken memories during sleep
In order to delve into this memory purging mechanism, researchers used an experimental technique to induce the memory activation artificially. Credit: Universidad de Barcelona

A study led by the University of Barcelona has identified the process in the brain which strengthens or weakens memories of daily events during sleep. According to the study published in the Journal of Neuroscience , the brain reactivates networks of interrelated memories during sleep, instead of doing so with isolated memories. Also, researchers show that in this memory reactivation process, the brain also promotes the forgetting of less important and less established information in the network of memories.

One of the most relevant questions in the field of is selecting experiences that are going to be remembered in the future. Scientific research studies have proved that during the sleeping process, the brain reactivates some of the encoded memories so that they are stored. In order to delve into this memory purging mechanism, used an experimental technique to induce the memory activation artificially.

This technique relates a sensory stimulation to a memory in order to induce its reactivation artificially during sleep. In the study, participants had to memorize fifteen locations –in a grid- of pairs of 147 identical cards with pictures of animals, means of transport or . In the first set, cards were in different positions inside the grid and the second set had one of the cards in a different place regarding the first one, while the other was in the same place. Therefore, the task made participants form two different sets of memory traces which shared a common element: the position of one of the cards in each pair. In addition, in the second set, card pairs were attached sounds related to the picture they had so that researchers could induce the memory activation for these positions showing sounds while participants were sleeping in the laboratory.

Therefore, while participants were sleeping, researchers showed half of the sound stimulus randomly selected (animal sounds, transport, or musical instruments) and reactivated memories of the positions of the cards that were related to each sound. However, when participants woke up, researchers evaluated the memory of the place of the first set of cards that had not been related to any sound but shared an element (they were related to each other) with those reactivated and those not reactivated during the sleeping process.

To manipulate the relation between the interrelated memories of the first set with those of the second set, researchers used two groups of participants. One group learnt both sets of cards consecutively (continuous group) so that both sets of cards were strongly related to each other. A second group learnt the second set of cards three hours after having learnt the first set (delay group). Therefore, both sets of memories were poorly related.

Network reactivated memories

Results revealed that the continuous group remembered better the memories of the first set of that were related to those that had been reactivated during sleep. That is, those reactivated memories through sounds, in the second set, helped them remembering the location of the related pairs in the first set (same picture and position of the first card). "In the first group, memories were strongly interrelated, since they were encoded in a very close and similar context. This feature allows these memories to benefit from the reactivation of other elements in the network, since they represent solid and important information", says the supervisor of the study, Lluís Fuentemilla.

Moreover, researchers found the opposite effect in the second group of participants. Positions of each card in the first set, which were related to those reactivated during sleep, were not remembered as much as those related with the ones that had not been reactivated with sounds. According to Javiera Oyarzún, first author of the article, "these results show for the first time that there is an active and selective mechanism to erase irrelevant or not frequent memories, which has been applied during sleep". Therefore, the reactivation of a specific memory, especially induced through sounds, has an impact on the memories of previously acquired similar experiences. In this case, memories with a strong relation benefited from the reactivation and were kept, while those that were less related to the reactivated memory were actively forgotten.

With this brain mechanism, the memory system would take the need of keeping information that can be useful in the future –according to the priority. "This strategy is highly adaptive, since it allows us to find patterns in the environment and predict what could happen if we find some situation which is similar to others we have already experienced. At the same time, it would prevent the memory of the information representing less frequent or inconsistent features from being kept in the long run, avoiding interferences in the memory in the future", says Lluís Fuentemilla.

Differences in electrophysiological activity

The study also analysed the brain electrophysiological activity of during memory reactivation and differences between the groups were detected. Records of both groups were placed in the theta activity during the reactivation process, but only the group with more forgetfulness recorded electrophysiological activity in the beta band. According to researchers, the fact that the same stimulation protocol during causes different oscillating activity could point that the brain involves two different neural mechanisms for memory strengthening and weakening. "The activity in the beta band would point the launch of an additional regulating neural mechanism to weaken those irrelevant memories and therefore keep the network of memories stable and efficient" says Javiera Oyarzún.

Explore further: Reactivating memories during sleep: Memory rehearsal during sleep can make a big difference in remembering later

More information: Javiera P. Oyarzún et al. Targeted Memory Reactivation during Sleep Adaptively Promotes the Strengthening or Weakening of Overlapping Memories, The Journal of Neuroscience (2017). DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3537-16.2017

Related Stories

Reactivating memories during sleep: Memory rehearsal during sleep can make a big difference in remembering later

April 12, 2013
Why do some memories last a lifetime while others disappear quickly? A new study suggests that memories rehearsed, during either sleep or waking, can have an impact on memory consolidation and on what is remembered later.

Finding traces of memory processing during sleep

May 17, 2017
Sleep helps us to retain the information that we have learned during the day. We know from animal experiments that new memories are reactivated during sleep. The brain replays previous experience while we sleep – and this ...

Memories can resist interference during new learning, study finds

May 26, 2016
While acquiring new memories can enrich the human experience, they also can interfere with old ones and make them more likely to be forgotten—especially when a new event is highly similar to a past experience.

Laser used to reawaken lost memories in mice with Alzheimer's disease

July 26, 2017
(Medical Xpress)—A team of researchers at Columbia University has found that applying a laser to the part of a mouse brain used for memory storage caused the mice to recall memories lost due to a mouse version of Alzheimer's ...

Long-term memories made with meaningful information

June 20, 2017
When trying to memorize information, it is better to relate it to something meaningful rather than repeat it again and again to make it stick, according to a recent Baycrest Health Sciences study published in NeuroImage.

A new technique isolates neuronal activity during memory consolidation

June 21, 2017
A team, led by researchers from the Cajal Institute (Madrid) belonging to the Spanish National Research Council (CSIC), have discovered some basic processes underlying memory consolidation in collaboration with colleagues ...

Recommended for you

Neural inflammation plays critical role in stress-induced depression

July 19, 2018
A group of Japanese researchers has discovered that neural inflammation caused by the innate immune system plays an unexpectedly important role in stress-induced depression. This insight could potentially lead to the development ...

Paralyzed mice with spinal cord injury made to walk again

July 19, 2018
Most people with spinal cord injury are paralyzed from the injury site down, even when the cord isn't completely severed. Why don't the spared portions of the spinal cord keep working? Researchers at Boston Children's Hospital ...

Scientists uncover the role of a protein in production and survival of myelin-forming cells

July 19, 2018
The nervous system is a complex organ that relies on a variety of biological players to ensure daily function of the human body. Myelin—a membrane produced by specialized glial cells—plays a critical role in protecting ...

Understanding the neuroscience of binge drinking

July 19, 2018
A new study from researchers at Columbia University Irving Medical Center found that binge drinking impairs working memory in the adolescent brain. The study, in mice, explains why teenagers who binge drink are 15 times more ...

Neurons can carry more than one signal at a time

July 18, 2018
Back in the early days of telecommunications, engineers devised a clever way to send multiple telephone calls through a single wire at the same time. Called time-division multiplexing, this technique rapidly switches between ...

Researchers solve mystery of how ALL enters the central nervous system

July 18, 2018
A deadly feature of acute lymphoblastic leukemia (ALL) is its invasion of the central nervous system.

1 comment

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

RobertKarlStonjek
not rated yet Oct 11, 2017
What evidence is there that memory is made up of 'memories', that is, separate and distinct mental entities (engrams)? This would be true of a computer, but for humans it is just an arbitrary assumption with no basis in fact beyond the observation that some kinds of recollections appear, at first glance, to be discrete. But in fact there are no discrete memories at all, not one (consider the relation of a memory to other memories, the properties of recognition ie that the memory has some meaning, context etc, the emotion or feeling associated with engrams and so on).

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.