Researchers discover link between gene variation and language

April 18, 2018, Brock University
Credit: CC0 Public Domain

What shapes the basic features of a language?

Brock University developmental psychologist Jan Frijters is part of an international, multi-disciplinary research team that found people whose mother tongue contains many consonants also carry a specific gene variation that helps them to distinguish a wide range of sounds.

The team, led by Jeffrey Gruen at Yale University, examined DCDC2, a gene strongly associated with the brain's capacity to process phonemes. A phoneme is a basic unit of sound that makes up words in a language, such as 'p' and 'b' in pit and bit. The number of phonemes varies from 20 to 60 per language.

The researchers examined the phonemes of languages spoken by 43 populations on five continents to see the extent to which play a role in subtle language differences.

They concentrated on a component of the DCDC2 gene called READ1, which has been consistently linked to dyslexia. The researchers studied how READ1 mutated over a four million-year period in nonhuman primates, Neanderthals, Denisovans and modern humans, and found that a variation of READ1 called RU1-1 was associated with variations in the number of consonants in a language.

There are more than 7,000 languages spoken globally, which are thought to have been shaped by migration, conquests and geographic isolation of populations, the researchers say.

However, this new study suggests that genetic factors also play a role in changes in language.

In determining this genetic connection, Frijters says the group—consisting of geneticists, a statistician, linguists, a pediatrician and a developmental psychologist—looked at three major characteristics of languages: tone, vowels and consonants.

"Think of a brain that has a lower capacity to discriminate specific types of sounds," says Frijters, an associate professor of Child and Youth Studies. "You need a language that has a small number of very distinct sounds or a low number of consonants."

In contrast, "a brain that can distinguish lots and lots of these types of sounds can handle a language that has very fine-grained distinctions between the consonants," he says.

"Of course, the amount of information that needs to be transmitted by every is constant, because we are all human, so languages that have fewer consonants pick up the information load in other ways."

Some languages—especially those in Asian and African countries—are highly tonal, using many pitches and inflections. In Mandarin Chinese, for instance, the word 'ma' has several different meanings depending on the tone used to say the word.

Languages also differ with the number of vowels, with high numbers in Estonian, Finnish and Danish.

But the team found that only consonants are associated with genetic variations, specifically the RU1-1 allele group of READ1, says the study.

"The brain uses distinct strategies to process and encode vowels and , which are modified by dyslexia genes. Genetic variations of these genes, along with other cultural and linguistic processes, may account for differences in consonant use between populations," the researchers conclude.

Yale University's Mellissa Demille is first author on the paper, "Worldwide distribution of the DCDC2 READ1 regulatory element and its relationship with phoneme variation across languages," which was published April 16 in the journal PNAS.

Explore further: Researchers unravel genetics of dyslexia and language impairment

More information: Mellissa M. C. DeMille et al. Worldwide distribution of the DCDC2 READ1 regulatory element and its relationship with phoneme variation across languages, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (2018). DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1710472115

Related Stories

Researchers unravel genetics of dyslexia and language impairment

June 13, 2013
A new study of the genetic origins of dyslexia and other learning disabilities could allow for earlier diagnoses and more successful interventions, according to researchers at Yale School of Medicine. Many students now are ...

Recommended for you

Identifying Crohn's disease risk factors in the Ashkenazi Jewish population

May 25, 2018
It is estimated that one in three individuals of Ashkenazi Jewish (AJ) descent carry mutations that increase their risk for certain genetic diseases. For instance, Crohn's, a highly heritable inflammatory bowel disease, is ...

How do insects survive on a sugary diet?

May 25, 2018
There's a reason parents tell their kids to lay off the sugar: too much isn't good for you.

Regulatory mutations missed in standard genetic screening lead to congenital diseases

May 25, 2018
Researchers have identified a type of genetic aberration to be the cause of certain neurodevelopmental disorders and congenital diseases, such as autism and congenital heart disease, which are undetectable by conventional ...

New chromosome study can lead to personalised counselling of pregnant women

May 25, 2018
Foetuses with a so-called new balanced chromosomal aberration have a higher risk of developing brain disorders such as autism and mental retardation than previously anticipated. The risk is 20 per cent for foetuses with these ...

New findings on autism-related disorder

May 24, 2018
In a study published today in Nature, Marc Bühler and his group at the Friedrich Miescher Institute for Biomedical Research (FMI) have taken a major step forward in elucidating the mechanisms underlying a disorder known ...

Genome study presents new way to track historical demographics of US populations

May 24, 2018
Sharon Browning of the University of Washington and colleagues developed a method to estimate historical effective population size, which is the number of individuals who pass on their genes to the next generation, to reveal ...

2 comments

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

Eikka
not rated yet Apr 18, 2018
"Of course, the amount of information that needs to be transmitted by every language is constant


Is it really?
wailuku1943
5 / 5 (1) Apr 18, 2018
Speaking as an anthropologist, I'd say "Yes, of course it is." You may perhaps be confusing certain kinds of complexity -- say, technological complexity -- with "amount of information" in general.

I can tell you that, having learned and spent a long time speaking a language that didn't include much technological complexity, the language in question has extraordinary levels of complexity. I can point to, for example, 63 pronouns and more than 50 "counting classes." In no case were these concepts that could not, with some effort, be talked about in English. English, though, is remarkably plain and simple in these areas.

So yes. The information content of human languages appears to be everywhere about the same. The complexities lie in different areas.

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.