Researchers map brain of blind patient who can see motion

June 12, 2018, University of Western Ontario
Neuropsychologist Jody Culham -- a neuropsychologist at the Brain and Mind Institute at Western University in London, Canada -- led researchers who discovered how a blind Scottish woman's brain adapted to be able to see objects in motion. Credit: Western University//London, Canada

Neuroscientists at Western University's Brain and Mind Institute, have confirmed and detailed a rare case of a blind woman able to see objects—but only if in motion.

A team led by neuropsychologist Jody Culham has conducted the most extensive analysis and to date of a blind patient, to help understand the remarkable vision of a 48-year-old Scottish woman, Milena Canning.

Canning lost her sight 18 years ago after a respiratory infection and series of strokes. Months after emerging blind from an eight-week coma, she was surprised to see the glint of a sparkly gift bag, like a flash of green lightning.

Then she began to perceive, sporadically, other moving things: her daughter's ponytail bobbing when she walked, but not her daughter's face; rain dripping down a window, but nothing beyond the glass; and water swirling down a drain, but not a tub already full with water.

Glaswegian ophthalmologist Gordon Dutton referred Canning to the Brain and Mind Institute in London, Canada, where tests by Culham's team included functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) to examine the real-time structure and workings of her brain.

They determined Canning has a rare phenomenon called Riddoch syndrome—in which a blind person can consciously see an object if moving but not if stationary.

"She is missing a piece of about the size of an apple at the back of her brain—almost her entire occipital lobes, which process vision," says Culham, a professor in the Department of Psychology and Graduate Program in Neuroscience.

"In Milena's case, we think the 'super-highway' for the visual system reached a dead end. But rather than shutting down her whole visual system, she developed some 'back roads' that could bypass the superhighway to bring some vision—especially motion—to other parts of the brain."

In essence, Canning's brain is taking unexpected, unconventional detours around damaged pathways.

During the study, Canning was able to recognize the motion, direction, size and speed of balls rolled towards her; and to command her hand to open, intercept and grab them at exactly the right time. She could navigate around chairs.

Yet she inconsistently identified an object's colour, and was able only half the time to detect whether someone's hand in front of her showed thumb-up or thumb-down.

"This work may be the richest characterization ever conducted of a single patient's visual system," says Culham. "She has shown this very profound recovery of vision, based on her perception of motion."

The research shows the remarkable plasticity of the human brain in finding work-arounds after catastrophic injuries. And it suggests conventional definitions of 'sight' and 'blindness' are fuzzier than previously believed.

"Patients like Milena give us a sense of what is possible and, even more importantly, they give us a sense of what visual and cognitive functions go together," Culham says.

This short clip shows brain scans of a sighted person, compared with the visual processing centers of Milena Canning. Credit: Brain and Mind Institute, Western University, Canada

For Canning, the research at BMI helps explain more about what she perceives and how her brain is continuing to change. She is able to navigate around chairs, can see a bright-shirted soccer goalie and can see steam rising from her morning cup of coffee, for example.

"I can't see like normal people see or like I used to see. The things I'm seeing are really strange. There is something happening and my is trying to rewire itself or trying different pathways," Canning says.

The research is newly published in the journal Neuropsychologia.

Explore further: Unique patient offers insights into the brain's quest to see

More information: Michael J. Arcaro et al, Psychophysical and neuroimaging responses to moving stimuli in a patient with the Riddoch phenomenon due to bilateral visual cortex lesions, Neuropsychologia (2018). DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.05.008

Related Stories

Unique patient offers insights into the brain's quest to see

June 7, 2018
When light streams through her window in just the right way, Milena Canning will sometimes stoop to pick up a shiny coin she has noticed on the wooden floor of her Glasgow-area home. But her hand comes up empty – the 'coin' ...

Blind people have brain map for 'visual' observations too

May 17, 2017
Is what you're looking at an object, a face, or a tree? When processing visual input, our brain uses different areas to recognize faces, body parts, scenes, and objects. Scientists at KU Leuven (University of Leuven), Belgium, ...

Retraining the brain to see after stroke

April 12, 2017
Patients who went partially blind after suffering a stroke regained large swaths of rudimentary sight after undergoing visual training designed by researchers at the University of Rochester Medical Center's Flaum Eye Institute.

Brains are more plastic than we thought

July 19, 2017
Practice might not always make perfect, but it's essential for learning a sport or a musical instrument. It's also the basis of brain training, an approach that holds potential as a non-invasive therapy to overcome disabilities ...

The mysterious case of the boy missing most of his visual cortex who can see anyway

December 8, 2017
(Medical Xpress)—A team of researchers with Monash University recently gave a presentation at a neuroscience conference in Australia outlining their study of the brain of a seven-year-old boy who was missing most of his ...

The human brain can 'see' what is around the corner

December 4, 2017
Neuroscientists at the University of Glasgow have shown how the human brain can predict what our eyes will see next, using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI).

Recommended for you

Attention, please! Anticipation of touch takes focus, executive skills

December 12, 2018
Anticipation is often viewed as an emotional experience, an eager wait for something to happen.

Study highlights potential benefits of continuous EEG monitoring for infant patients

December 12, 2018
A recent retrospective study evaluating continuous electroencephalography (cEEG) of children in intensive care units (ICUs) found a higher than anticipated number of seizures. The work also identified several conditions closely ...

The importins of anxiety

December 11, 2018
According to some estimates, up to one in three people around the world may experience severe anxiety in their lifetime. In a study described today in Cell Reports, researchers at the Weizmann Institute of Science have revealed ...

How returning to a prior context briefly heightens memory recall

December 11, 2018
Whether it's the pleasant experience of returning to one's childhood home over the holidays or the unease of revisiting a site that proved unpleasant, we often find that when we return to a context where an episode first ...

Neurons in the brain work as a team to guide movement of arms, hands

December 11, 2018
The apparent simplicity of picking up a cup of coffee or turning a doorknob belies the complex sequence of calculations and processes that the brain must undergo to identify the location of an item in space, move the arm ...

Using neurofeedback to prevent PTSD in soldiers

December 11, 2018
A team of researchers from Israel, the U.S. and the U.K. has found that using neurofeedback could prevent soldiers from experiencing PTSD after engaging in emotionally difficult situations. In their paper published in the ...

3 comments

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

sparcboy
not rated yet Jun 12, 2018
Hhhmmm .... in the book "Phantoms of the Brain" it was shown we have an old visual system that is not tied to the occipital lobes, but a deeper part of the brain.

Could that play a part? Maybe not, if I remember correctly that part did not render vision to consciousness.
RobertKarlStonjek
not rated yet Jun 13, 2018
It seems to me that this is an obvious case of one processing area remaining in tact and that motions is separated out and processed independently of the rest of the visual field.

This idea is strengthened by another rare condition where a patient's vision is normal *except* for motion. Blindness to motion can only be explained if motion is separated out and processed independently. Such a person will see their coffee cup empty and then suddenly full when filling it up or a car at a distance suddenly appearing close but in both cases apparently motionless.

I'd be extremely surprised if these two types of cases were not indicative of the same phenomena, that of independent processing of motion, either being the only process missing or the only process remaining, as in the above mentioned case :)
Ojorf
not rated yet Jun 13, 2018
It seems to me that this is an obvious case of one processing area remaining in tact and that motions is separated out and processed independently of the rest of the visual field.

Yes.
The visual signals received by the the brain are already pre-processed in the retina and separated. Motion (separately for different directions), edge detection (separately for edges between different colours), colour and more are all received separately and processed separately in the brain.

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.