To understand working memory, scientists must resolve this debate

August 8, 2018, Massachusetts Institute of Technology
Earl Miller lectures about working memory at a recent symposium at MIT. Credit: Adrianne Mathiowetz/Picower Institute at MIT

In a debate where the stakes are nothing short of understanding how the brain maintains its "sketchpad of conscious thought," researchers argue over exactly what makes working memory work in dueling papers in the Aug. 8 edition of the Journal of Neuroscience.

Working is how you hold things in mind like the directions to a new restaurant and the list of specials the waiter rattles off after you sit down. Given that capacity is a strong correlate of intelligence and that its dysfunction is a major symptom in common psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia and autism, it's important that the field achieve a true understanding of how it works, said Mikael Lundqvist, a postdoc at MIT's Picower Institute for Learning and Memory and lead author of one of the papers.

"Working memory deficits are associated with virtually every major psychiatric disorder, but if we can figure out how working memory works, we can figure out how to fix it," added corresponding author and Picower Professor Earl Miller. "Working memory is the sketchpad of consciousness. Doesn't everyone want to know how our conscious mind works?"

The opposing "Dual Perspectives" paper in the journal is led by Christos Constantinidis of the Wake Forest School of Medicine.

Sharp divisions on spiking

The central issue of the debate is what happens after you hear or see what you need to remember and must then hold or control it in mind to apply it later. During that interim, or "delay period," do in your brain's prefrontal cortex maintain it by persistently firing away, like an idling car engine, or do they spike in brief but coordinated bursts to store and retrieve information via the patterns of their connections, akin to how longer-term memory works?

In their essay, Lundqvist, Miller and Pawel Herman off the KTH Royal Institute of Technology in Stockholm take the latter position. They argue that brief, coordinated bursts are clearly evident in the observations of the most recent experiments and that such activity can more satisfactorily produce key attributes of working memory, including efficient, independent control of multiple items with precise timing.

Importantly, Miller said, the idea that spiking during the delay period drives changes in neural connections, or synapses, reinforces the classic idea that spiking has a crucial role in working memory. The disagreement, he says, is merely that the spiking activity is not as persistent as it looks in older experiments.

"We're showing additional mechanisms by which spiking maintains working memory and gives volitional control," Miller said. "Our work doesn't argue against the idea that delay activity spiking plays a role in working memory, it adds further support. We are just saying that at a more granular level, there are some additional things going on."

For example, much of the disagreement arises from how different researchers have collected and analyzed their data. The data supporting the persistence interpretation arise mostly from analyses in which researchers averaged the firing patterns of small numbers of neurons over many working memory trials, the MIT authors said.

Averages, however, tend to smooth data out over the long term. Instead, in newer experiments, scientists have analyzed the spiking of many neurons in each individual trial. There, it's clear that as animals perform working memory tasks, populations of neurons fire in brief, coordinated bursts, Miller and Lundqvist said.

In their research, members of the Miller lab have also shown how groups of neurons are coordinated, demonstrating how a large-scale, precisely timed interplay of brain rhythms correlate with goal-directed control of working memory functions such as storing or releasing, information from being in mind.

Some of the disagreement arises from models of working . That neurons fire in short, cohesive bursts in accord with circuit-wide oscillations, makes functional sense, Miller and Lundqvist argue. It uses less energy than keeping neurons firing all the time, for example, and readily explains how multiple items can be held in mind simultaneously (distinct bursts representing different pieces of information can occur at different times). Moreover, storing information in patterns of synaptic connections makes the information more resilient to distraction than if neurons are constantly trying to maintain it through activity.

"Storing information with a mixture of spiking and synapses gives the brain more flexibility," Lundqvist said. "It can juggle the activation of different memories, allowing the brain to hold multiple memories without them interfering with each other. Plus, synapses can store temporarily store memories while the spiking processes other thoughts.

"This could explain how our working memory is not erased by things that temporarily distract us," he said.

Got to get it settled

With a lot of new research activity and data coming in, Lundqvist added, it's a debate whose time has come.

"This is a good time to see what the evidence is and to determine what are the experiments that will settle this," he said. "We need more experiments to settle this. They will give us not only more insight into this question of persistence but also about working memory function."

To help research continue to move forward, the MIT paper recommends four major principles: Measuring the activity of whole populations of individual neurons; analyzing every trial separately; making the tasks animals do complex enough to require controlling multiple pieces of information; and measuring neural rhythms, not just spiking.

Explore further: A new glimpse into working memory

More information: Journal of Neuroscience (2018). DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2485-17.2018

Related Stories

A new glimpse into working memory

March 17, 2016
When you hold in mind a sentence you have just read or a phone number you're about to dial, you're engaging a critical brain system known as working memory.

Neuroscientists suggest a model for how we gain volitional control of what we hold in our minds

January 16, 2018
Working memory is a sort of "mental sketchpad" that allows you to accomplish everyday tasks such as calling in your hungry family's takeout order and finding the bathroom you were just told "will be the third door on the ...

A heavy working memory load may sink brainwave 'synch'

April 5, 2018
Everyday experience makes it obvious - sometimes frustratingly so - that our working memory capacity is limited. We can only keep so many things consciously in mind at once. The results of a new study may explain why: They ...

New study explains how your brain helps you learn new skills

February 8, 2018
Even if you haven't ridden your bike in years, you probably remember how to do so without giving it much thought. If you're a skilled piano player, odds are you can easily sit down and play a song you've rehearsed before. ...

Researchers unravel new insights into how the brain beats distractions to retain memories

October 31, 2017
Researchers from the National University of Singapore (NUS) have recently discovered a mechanism that could explain how the brain retains working memory when faced with distractions. These findings could endow cognitive flexibility ...

Receptors key to strong memories

February 27, 2018
When we create a memory, a pattern of connections forms between neurons in the brain. New work from UC Davis shows how these connections can be strengthened or weakened at a molecular level. The study is published Feb. 27 ...

Recommended for you

Brain response study upends thinking about why practice speeds up motor reaction times

August 16, 2018
Researchers in the Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation at Johns Hopkins Medicine report that a computerized study of 36 healthy adult volunteers asked to repeat the same movement over and over became significantly ...

Newly identified role for inhibition in cerebellar plasticity and behavior

August 16, 2018
Almost everyone is familiar with the unique mixture of surprise and confusion that occurs after making a mistake during an everyday movement. It's a fairly startling experience—stumbling on a step or accidentally missing ...

Men and women show surprising differences in seeing motion

August 16, 2018
Researchers reporting in the journal Current Biology on August 16 have found an unexpected difference between men and women. On average, their studies show, men pick up on visual motion significantly faster than women do.

How people use, and lose, preexisting biases to make decisions

August 16, 2018
From love and politics to health and finances, humans can sometimes make decisions that appear irrational, or dictated by an existing bias or belief. But a new study from Columbia University neuroscientists uncovers a surprisingly ...

Working memory might be more flexible than previously thought

August 16, 2018
Breaking with the long-held idea that working memory has fixed limits, a new study by researchers at Uppsala University and New York University suggests that these limits adapt themselves to the task that one is performing. ...

Protein droplets keep neurons at the ready and immune system in balance

August 15, 2018
Inside cells, where DNA is packed tightly in the nucleus and rigid proteins keep intricate transport systems on track, some molecules have a simpler way of establishing order. They can self-organize, find one another in crowded ...

0 comments

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.