Hallucinations induced in lab could be key to better understanding and treatment

Hallucinations induced in lab could be key to better understanding and treatment
Hallucinations have been difficult to study and can be distressing for the person experiencing them, but hallucinations induced in the lab are much more benign. Credit: Shutterstock

Cognitive neuroscientists from UNSW Sydney say if we really want to understand and treat the pathological hallucinations that affect people with physical and mental illnesses, the best place to start is in the laboratory.

Inducing in the using visual stimulation procedures works similarly to illusions, and enables more objective and repeatable testing. It's also much less distressing to the test subject than studying pathological hallucinations experienced by people with conditions like Parkinson's disease or schizophrenia.

"By nature, [lab-induced hallucinations] can be induced in almost anyone at any time," the neuroscientists write in an opinion piece published recently in Philosophical Transactions B journal.

"This can help to curb the current overreliance on studying pathological hallucinations, thereby reducing burdens placed on patients and simplifying recruitment and testing logistics."

Seeing something that isn't there

Most people naturally think of visual hallucinations as being realistic images or scenes, such as seeing humans or spiders (what we call 'complex' hallucinations). However, a hallucination in its broadest sense can be defined as the experience of seeing something that is not there. As such, visual hallucinations can also include seeing basic geometric shapes or colors (referred to as 'simple' hallucinations), and scientists can trigger both simple and complex hallucinations in the laboratory.

Professor Joel Pearson, the senior author of the opinion piece, says work the group did in 2016 showed that you could induce hallucinations in people reliably and safely using specific types of flickering lights.

"We showed that you could use flickering lights in an annulus—basically a flickering white ring like a doughnut on a —and you could induce hallucinations of little dark blobs which rotate around the ring," he says.

"And you could use that to try and study the mechanisms behind visual hallucinations. But those flicker hallucinations are just the tip of the iceberg, and there are many other techniques for inducing hallucinations that are similar to pathological hallucinations in terms of the experience and underlying neural processes."

Prof. Pearson says one of the trickier problems is working out which techniques can tell us something about pathological hallucinations.

"A lot of this work shows that it's hard to separate hallucinations from illusions and veridical (reality-based) perception. Current hallucination definitions are too black and white, and aren't up to the task of classifying many of these lab-induced experiences."

Spectrum of experience

Professor Pearson and fellow authors Dr. Sebastian Rogers and Dr. Rebecca Keogh use a continuous spectrum of experience to distinguish hallucinations from other types of perception, based on the similarity between the physical stimulation of the senses (the light that enters the eye) and the actual conscious experience (the image we 'see' or experience).

Veridical perception (involving a between what is 'in reality' and what one sees) is at one end of this spectrum and hallucinations (a weak relationship between what is present in reality and with what one sees) is at the other, with illusions falling somewhere in between.

According to lead author Dr. Rogers, "the thesis of the idea is that the further a lab-induced experience is toward the hallucination end of the spectrum, the more it can tell us about other types of hallucination."

"If you really don't want to call one of these lab-induced experiences a hallucination, that's fine by us. We don't really mind what the name is, we care most about whether we can study it to learn about pathological and other hallucinations. It's a way to investigate hallucinatory processes any time we want in the lab, with anyone."

Dr. Keogh says, "once we understand the underlying mechanisms, that is, what in the brain leads to seeing things that aren't there, then we'll be able to develop treatments. There are very few treatments for hallucinations at the moment, and most are medications that can lead to unwanted side effects.

"Using lab hallucination models can allow us to develop new avenues for more targeted treatments, such as electrical or magnetic brain stimulation."


Explore further

Breakthrough opens door to study hallucinations scientifically

More information: Sebastian Rogers et al. Hallucinations on demand: the utility of experimentally induced phenomena in hallucination research, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences (2020). DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2020.0233
Citation: Hallucinations induced in lab could be key to better understanding and treatment (2020, December 15) retrieved 15 June 2021 from https://medicalxpress.com/news/2020-12-hallucinations-lab-key-treatment.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.
32 shares

Feedback to editors

User comments