A small region of the brain called the primary visual cortex can be targeted using a virus (light green) to block habituation learning. Credit: Eitan S. Kaplan and Sam F. Cooke/Bear Lab

In findings that may lead to new treatments for cognitive disorders, researchers at MIT's Picower Institute for Learning and Memory zero in on how the brain forms memories of what has been seen.

In a paper appearing this week in the online edition of Nature Neuroscience, a research team led by Mark Bear, the Picower Professor of Neuroscience, showed that dramatic changes occur in the when mice learn to distinguish novel from familiar visual stimuli. Manipulations that prevented the changes in visual cortex also blocked formation.

Impairments in detecting and recognizing familiar visual elements and patterns are features of a number of neuropsychiatric disorders, including autism and schizophrenia. With these new findings, "we now have an opportunity to investigate how gene mutations that cause or increase the risk for these disorders disrupt the mechanisms of visual recognition memory," says Bear, a Howard Hughes Medical Institute investigator. "We anticipate that this knowledge will suggest entirely novel approaches to treating these diseases."

To understand the physical basis of memory, researchers seek to identify where and how the brain changes as learning occurs—something that has been very difficult to achieve.

The study's lead author, Picower Institute research scientist Samuel F. Cooke, working with postdoctoral fellows Robert W. Komorowski and Jeffrey Gavornik and graduate student Eitan S. Kaplan, showed that mice move to investigate a that has never before been experienced, but stop moving when the same stimulus becomes familiar. They discovered that as familiarity was learned, synaptic transmission was changed in the primary visual cortex. Preventing or reversing this in visual cortex left the animals unable to distinguish familiar and novel visual stimuli.

Previously, the primary visual cortex was seen as a "first responder" to visual stimuli that quickly passes information along to higher-order brain regions for interpretation and memory storage. "The study points to the visual cortex as a tool of learning and memory in its own right, capable of storing simple but fundamentally important memories," Cooke says. "Our work provides great hope for the future as it suggests we may have the chance to directly observe neurons undergo lasting changes as a very simple and experimentally constrained memory is formed."

Bear anticipates that the results will surprise neuroscientists. "We find that, contrary to the dogma that the primary is relatively immutable in adults, a form of induces synaptic modifications in this area, and these modifications are necessary for a type of visual recognition memory."

More information: "Visual recognition memory, manifested as long-term habituation, requires synaptic plasticity in V1." Nature Neuroscience (2015) DOI: 10.1038/nn.3920

Journal information: Nature Neuroscience