Flashbulb memories – why do we remember learning about dramatic events so vividly?

April 16, 2015 by Jennifer Talarico, The Conversation
Flashbulb memories aren’t set in stone. Credit: Jan, CC BY-NC-SA

It isn't surprising that many Bostonians have vivid memories of the 2013 Marathon bombing, or that many New Yorkers have very clear memories about where they were and what they were doing on 9/11.

But many individuals who were not onsite for these attacks, or not even in Boston on April 15 2013 or in New York on September 11 2001 also have vivid memories of how they learned about these . Why would people who were not immediately or directly affected have such a long-lasting sense of knowing exactly where they were and what they were doing when they heard the news?

These recollections are called flashbulb memories. In a flashbulb memory, we recall the experience of learning about an event, not the factual details of the event itself.

There might be an advantage to recalling the elements of important events that happen to us or to those close to us, but there appears to be little benefit to recalling our experience hearing this kind of news. So why does learning about a big event create such vivid memories? And just how accurate are flashbulb memories?

Strong emotions and personal connections

Not all historical events lead to flashbulb memories. An event must capture our individual attention and be identified as something significant before the memory is intensified. In order for us to exhibit this enhanced memory phenomenon, it seems critical that we feel a sense of personal or cultural connection to the event that results in a strong emotional reaction.

Hearing that a loved one has unexpectedly died would likely lead to a flashbulb-like memory, however, psychologists tend to study public events so they can examine a large number of memories referencing the same event. By doing so, investigators can examine the memories from a large group of individuals, often at varying intervals, to see how memories change over time.

Cross-cultural studies of flashbulb memories show that although the types of events and the memories that result are quite similar from person to person, the specific events that lead to these memories vary dramatically.

For instance, the 1977 study that coined the term "flashbulb memories" showed that although both black and white Americans almost universally recalled flashbulb memories of John F Kennedy's assassination, black Americans were more than twice as likely to have flashbulb memories for the assassination of Martin Luther King, Jr than were white Americans.

Some theorists have argued that part of the reason that our flashbulb memories are so long-lasting is because having such a vivid memory is "proof" of our membership in a particular social group. It would be a poor patriot who could not remember what he or she was doing on September 11 2001.

Flashbulb memories – why do we remember learning about dramatic events so vividly?
John F Kennedy’s funeral, November 24 1963. Credit: The US National Archives
Flashbulb memories have vivid details

The first description of flashbulb-type memories in the psychological literature (by F W Colgrove in 1899) is actually of Abraham Lincoln's assassination and the sample report includes abundant, specific detail:

Everybody looked so sad, and there was such terrible excitement that my father stopped his horse, and leaning from the carriage called: 'What is it my friends? What has happened?' 'Haven't you heard?' was their reply–'Lincoln has been assassinated.' The lines fell from my father's limp hands, and with tears streaming from his eyes he sat as one bereft of motion.

Although we can remember many events from our lives for decades or longer, it's the particular ease with which these extremely come to mind after lengthy, sometimes lifelong delays, that also makes them remarkable.

Compared to ordinary , flashbulb memories include richer sensory detail. For example, you may readily be able to picture people and places clearly and to hear the sounds of voices and ambient noises intensely. These memories are also characterized by the presence of "idiosyncratic details" that seem to be irrelevant to the overall scene.

More details, but not necessarily accurate details

Martin Luther King, Jr at the Civil Rights March on Washington, DC, August 28 1963. Credit: The US National Archives

Because we can easily recall a lot of details about the event, we believe those details accurately reflect what happened. But it turns out that the durability and the vividness of these memories are actually more reliable than their accuracy. In other words, although we feel like we remember exactly where we were and what we were doing, the evidence suggests that our confidence may be misplaced.

Have you ever disagreed with a spouse or a sibling about what actually happened at an event you both attended? You might realize that our memories are not a perfect reproduction of what occurred in the past. Instead, psychologists describe memories as being reconstructions of the past. Memories are based, in part, on what actually happened (obviously), but are also influenced by our current thoughts and emotions and our reasons for remembering.

You don't remember events exactly as they happened

All memories tend to lose detail over time and we sometimes confuse details from one event with those from another. This is also true of flashbulb memories. We are just as prone to forgetting and, more interestingly, potentially more vulnerable to mis-remembering, flashbulb memories than other autobiographical memories. Because we frequently think about and talk about our flashbulb memories, we sometimes add details from other events or incorporate details suggested by others. By doing so, we shape our memories into a coherent, interesting story to share.

Media coverage contributes, in part, to this phenomenon. Repeatedly viewing footage that was only available later can sometimes lead to mistakenly remembering that you saw those images at the time of the event. The media can also serve as a cue to think about or talk about these memories, enhancing their accessibility and vividness.

So, although we have a subjective feeling of remembering these events "exactly" as they occurred, this is typically not the case. When people are asked to record their memories, these objective reports for flashbulb memories include errors of omission and commission to the same degree as other autobiographical memories from the same time.

Why, then, do we feel like we remember exactly where we were and what we were doing when we learned of an important event? Because doing so demonstrates to ourselves and to others what we believe to be important.

Explore further: Brian Williams told a tale – but it could be how he really remembers what happened

Related Stories

Brian Williams told a tale – but it could be how he really remembers what happened

February 13, 2015
Many of us have asked ourselves in the past few days: can you really falsely remember something as significant as being in a helicopter that was shot down? And many of us probably think "No way," and quickly conclude that ...

3 Questions: John Gabrielli on studying traumatic memories

September 9, 2011
Starting just days after the Sept. 11, 2001, terrorist attacks, MIT neuroscientist John Gabrieli (who was then at Stanford University) and colleagues around the country undertook a large-scale survey of how people remembered ...

How the brain recombines memories of past events when complemented by new information

April 1, 2015
Scientists from Radboud University have visualized - for the first time - how the brain recombines memories of past events when these are complemented by new information. Scientific paper Current Biology published their results ...

Study finds that learning by repetition impairs recall of details

June 23, 2014
When learning, practice doesn't always make perfect.

How our emotions transform mundane events into strong memories

April 6, 2015
Human beings are information seekers. We are constantly taking in details – big and small – from our environment. But the majority of the stuff we encounter in a given day we rarely need to remember. For instance, what ...

Recommended for you

Self-perception and reality seem to line-up when it comes to judging our own personality

December 14, 2018
When it comes to self-assessment, new U of T research suggests that maybe we do have a pretty good handle on our own personalities after all.

Levels of gene-expression-regulating enzyme altered in brains of people with schizophrenia

December 14, 2018
A study using a PET scan tracer developed at the Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging at Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH) has identified, for the first time, epigenetic differences between the brains of individuals ...

Researchers discover abundant source for neuronal cells

December 13, 2018
USC researchers seeking a way to study genetic activity associated with psychiatric disorders have discovered an abundant source of human cells—the nose.

Video game players frequently exposed to graphic content may see world differently

December 13, 2018
People who frequently play violent video games are more immune to disturbing images than non-players, a UNSW-led study into the phenomenon of emotion-induced blindness has shown.

New genetic clues to early-onset form of dementia

December 13, 2018
Unlike the more common Alzheimer's disease, frontotemporal dementia tends to afflict young people. It accounts for an estimated 20 percent of all cases of early-onset dementia. Patients with the illness typically begin to ...

How teens deal with stress may affect their blood pressure, immune system

December 13, 2018
Most teens get stressed out by their families from time to time, but whether they bottle those emotions up or put a positive spin on things may affect certain processes in the body, including blood pressure and how immune ...

2 comments

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

RobertKarlStonjek
not rated yet Apr 17, 2015
Once again I'm amazed at how ignorant scientists can be when they step out of their own discipline...

It has already been established empirically that the process of 'reconsolidation' stridently secures memory engrams and that simply recalling and relating the event to others numerous times is sufficient for this reconsolidation to occur.

So if we consider the *behaviour* of individuals at the time they learn about events that result in flashbulb memories the bulk of the mystery is solved: people talk to each other, recalling the events and in the process unwittingly consolidate the memory.

We need to consider social scientific data, psychology and cognitive science and the picture becomes perfectly clear and demystified. Who are blacks likely to discuss more among themselves: Kennedy or King? Mystery solved.
RobertKarlStonjek
not rated yet Apr 17, 2015
But if you ignored people's actual behaviour and just asked about the brain and especially if you made the erroneous assumption that hearing of the initial event caused something different to occur in the brain then you are off on a wild goose chase.

For instance we often here of devastating news that turns out to be a rumour or someone simply miss-heard a radio news bulletin. If we go with the neuroscience model of flash bulb memory then all of those should also be be prominent in memory, but they are not...why aren't they?

When humans chat with each other they are unwittingly determining what is important and worth remembering and what can be dismissed as irrelevant to the group (Evolutionary Psychology would tell you that, but who bothers to look outside their own discipline for answers??? I think the time has come!!!!!)

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.