This article has been reviewed according to Science X's editorial process and policies. Editors have highlighted the following attributes while ensuring the content's credibility:

fact-checked

peer-reviewed publication

trusted source

proofread

'Smart' drugs can decrease productivity in people who don't have ADHD, study finds

child studying
Credit: Pixabay/CC0 Public Domain

New research from the University of Cambridge and the University of Melbourne, published in Science Advances, shows neurotypical workers and students taking cognitive enhancers, or "smart" drugs, may actually be inhibiting their performance and productivity.

Drugs such as methylphenidate, sold under the brand name Ritalin among others, are commonly prescribed for (ADHD), but are also taken by those without a diagnosis, in the belief that the drugs will enhance focus and cognitive performance.

In four double-blinded, randomized trials in Melbourne, each a week apart, the same 40 healthy participants took one of three popular "smart" drugs (methylphenidate, modafinil or dextroamphetamine) or a placebo. They were assessed on how they performed in a test designed to model the complex decision-making and problem-solving present in our everyday lives.

While previous studies into the effects of smart drugs have used simpler targeting memory or attention, the Melbourne trial involved more computationally complex activities that better simulate the difficult nature of tasks people encounter in daily life.

Participants were asked to complete an exercise known as the Knapsack Optimization Problem—or "knapsack task"—in which they were given a virtual knapsack with a set capacity, and a selection of items of different weights and values. The participants had to figure out how to best allocate items to the bag, to maximize the overall value of its contents.

Overall, participants taking the drugs saw small decreases in accuracy and efficiency, along with large increases in time and effort, relative to their results when not taking the drugs.

For example, when given methylphenidate—often used to treat ADHD in children, but increasingly taken by cramming for exams—participants took around 50% longer on average to complete the knapsack problem as when they were given a placebo.

In addition, participants who performed at a higher level in the placebo condition compared to the rest of the group tended to show a bigger decrease in performance and productivity after receiving a .

In terms of "productivity," for example—the level of progress per item moved in or out of the knapsack—the participants in the top 25% under a placebo regularly ended up in the bottom 25% under methylphenidate.

By contrast, participants who had a lower performance in a placebo condition only very occasionally exhibited a slight improvement after taking a drug.

Professor Peter Bossaerts, Leverhulme International Professor of Neuroeonomics at the University of Cambridge, believes more research needs to be conducted to find out what effects the drugs are having on users without ADHD.

"Our results suggest that these drugs don't actually make you 'smarter,'" said Bossaerts. "Because of the dopamine the drugs induce, we expected to see increased motivation, and they do motivate one to try harder. However, we discovered that this exertion caused more erratic thinking—in ways that we could make precise because the knapsack task had been widely studied in computer science."

"Performance did not generally increase, so questions remain about how the drugs are affecting people's minds and their decision making."

Dr. Elizabeth Bowman researcher at the Centre for Brain, Mind and Markets at the University of Melbourne and lead author of the study said the results show we have yet to establish the effectiveness of pharmaceutical enhancers on our performance, when used by neurotypical people to perform everyday complex tasks.

"Our research shows drugs that are expected to improve in patients may actually be leading to healthy users working harder while producing a lower quality of work in a longer amount of time," said Bowman.

More information: Elizabeth Bowman et al, Not so smart? "Smart" drugs increase the level but decrease the quality of cognitive effort, Science Advances (2023). DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.add4165. www.science.org/doi/10.1126/sciadv.add4165

Journal information: Science Advances
Citation: 'Smart' drugs can decrease productivity in people who don't have ADHD, study finds (2023, June 14) retrieved 27 April 2024 from https://medicalxpress.com/news/2023-06-smart-drugs-decrease-productivity-people.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further

ADHD: Emotional problems may be the missing link

74 shares

Feedback to editors