A study in adaptability: Why do we change our beliefs?

October 9, 2012

(Medical Xpress)—The human brain likes to make predictions about how the world works. Imagine, for example, that you move to a new town. At first, you don't know where to go for dinner. But after weeks of trying different restaurants, you pick a favorite, a little Thai place that makes the best green curry. Several months later, however, you notice the curry isn't as spicy and the vegetables seem undercooked. At first you give your favorite place the benefit of the doubt. But after a few more so-so dinners, you suddenly realize that something must have changed—perhaps the owner hired a new chef—and your notion that this is the best place around is no longer valid. So you begin searching for a new favorite restaurant.

Neuroscientists have long been interested in this adaptability, particularly in the moment when an individual discards an old belief and begins to formulate a new one. "You go from being confident in your model of the world to being uncertain and then abandoning the model altogether," says Alla Karpova, a group leader at the Howard Hughes Medical Institute's Janelia Farm Research Campus. She and her colleagues wondered what goes on in the brain when this happens. In rats, they found that the rejection of an old belief correlates with abrupt changes in activity in the medial prefrontal cortex, a brain region involved in cognitive functions such as reward anticipation and decision-making. The team's research is published in the October 5, 2012, issue of Science.

To examine this moment of transition in rats, the researchers designed a experiment that allowed them to differentiate between changes in due to a change in from those due to a change in the animal's internal state—its belief. The key, Karpova says, was to use a task with stochastic reward, so that if the animal completes the task once without receiving a reward, that is not enough to conclude that the reward probability has changed. Instead, the results of multiple trials must be combined to reach that conclusion.

To set up the experiment, they placed a wall with three holes just big enough for a rat nose at one end of a cage. When the rats poked their noses in the middle hole, one of two tones sounded and handles appeared above the left and right holes. The rats quickly figured out that when they heard the first tone, they should pull the left handle to receive a tasty morsel of food. When they heard the second tone, they should pull the right handle. "When animals enter a novel environment, they just try different things out and can learn pretty complicated sequences of actions to get rewards," Karpova says. Pulling the wrong handle, they discovered, meant no treat and a time out.

But not every handle pull resulted in a treat. What's more, the handles weren't equal. Pulling the right lever rarely resulted in a treat. Pulling the left one often did. When the rats realized this, they stopped pulling the right handle altogether. If the tone associated with the right handle sounded, they simply nuzzled the middle hole to reset the experiment. The rat became "convinced that the left side was much better and confidently rejected right-side trials," Karpova says.

After a certain number of trials, however, the researchers mixed things up. They turned the tables so that the right handle often resulted in a reward while the left rarely did. At first the rats kept pressing the lever that used be more likely to give a reward. But after a while, they started to explore both sides, having realized that something was amiss. "What suddenly happens is they go into this mode where they explore both options," Karpova says. Over time the rats began to favor the now–better option.

Karpova and her colleagues wanted to examine what happens inside the rat's brain at the moment of uncertainty. So they used electrodes to record the activity of neurons in the medial prefrontal cortex. "If this area is involved in encoding a model, then at the moment when you decide to discard your old model, things should abruptly change," she says.

Since the underlying change in confidence was the only thing that changed abruptly at such a moment in their experiment, a simultaneous in activity in the brain could -be attributed to the rat's decision to abandon its old belief. And that's exactly what the researchers observed. When the rats seemed certain which handle they should pull, activity in the medial prefrontal cortex was relatively stable. But during the crucial moment of the onset of uncertainty, when the rat reverted to pulling both handles, "the activity abruptly and markedly changed and then remained more variable for the duration of the period when the animal sampled both options," Karpova says. "It's as if those neurons were the ones searching for the animal's new model."

The results suggest that the researchers have found the circuitry monitoring and possibly building a cognitive world—at least in rats—but they don't provide definitive proof. The ideal next experiment, Karpova says, would involve perturbing the circuitry to create a similar change in brain activity. If the researchers could show that the change makes the rats more uncertain and willing to explore, that would provide stronger evidence. Karpova also hopes to identify the trigger that sparks these changes in activity. She and her colleagues suspect it might be the neurotransmitter norepinephrine. If they're right, they can use it to manipulate the activity in the in just the right way to determine causality.

The research provides with a better understanding of the brain's capabilities, and Karpova speculates it may also help explain some neurological disorders. For example, people who have obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) show altered activity in the same region the researchers examined in the . "It's not entirely unreasonable to think that part of the issue, at least in some forms [of OCD], is an inability to get rid of your beliefs," Karpova says. "If you believe your hands are dirty, you'll keep washing them and no evidence will be sufficient to let you discard that belief."

Explore further: Study offers clues as to why teens are more susceptible to addiction and mental illness

More information: www.hhmi.org/research/groupleaders/karpova.html

Related Stories

Study offers clues as to why teens are more susceptible to addiction and mental illness

January 17, 2012
(Medical Xpress) -- Researchers at the University of Pittsburgh’s Moghaddam Laboratory, led by biochemist Bita Moghaddam have found after studying rat brains that minor differences in activity levels in certain brain ...

Aging brain gets stuck in time, researchers show

March 14, 2012
The aging brain loses its ability to recognize when it is time to move on to a new task, explaining why the elderly have difficulty multi-tasking, Yale University researchers report.

What do animals 'know'? More than you may think

December 9, 2011
(PhysOrg.com) -- Rats use their knowledge to make decisions when faced with ambiguous situations, UCLA psychologists report.

Researchers provide exciting first glimpse into the competitive brain

August 13, 2012
(Medical Xpress) -- While most of us have been wrapped up in the competitive spirit of the Olympic Games, two University of Otago researchers have been busy teasing out what exactly in the brain drives competitive behaviour.

Recommended for you

Offbeat brain rhythms during sleep make older adults forget

December 15, 2017
Like swinging a tennis racket during a ball toss to serve an ace, slow and speedy brainwaves during deep sleep must sync up at exactly the right moment to hit the save button on new memories, according to new UC Berkeley ...

Our memory shifts into high gear when we think about raising our children, new study shows

December 15, 2017
Human memory has evolved so people better recall events encountered while they are thinking about raising their offspring, according to a new study conducted by researchers at Binghamton University, State University of New ...

Study finds graspable objects grab attention more than images of objects do

December 15, 2017
Does having the potential to act upon an object have a unique influence on behavior and brain responses to the object? That is the question Jacqueline Snow, assistant professor of psychology at the University of Nevada, Reno, ...

Little understood cell helps mice see color

December 14, 2017
Researchers at the University of Colorado Anschutz Medical Campus have discovered that color vision in mice is far more complex than originally thought, opening the door to experiments that could potentially lead to new treatments ...

Scientists chart how brain signals connect to neurons

December 14, 2017
Scientists at Johns Hopkins have used supercomputers to create an atomic scale map that tracks how the signaling chemical glutamate binds to a neuron in the brain. The findings, say the scientists, shed light on the dynamic ...

Journaling inspires altruism through an attitude of gratitude

December 14, 2017
Gratitude does more than help maintain good health. New research at the University of Oregon finds that regularly noting feelings of gratitude in a journal leads to increased altruism.

0 comments

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.