New experimental method allows spontaneous synchronization of arm motions by pairs of Japanese macaques

May 2, 2013
Figure 1: The solo, paired, and video playback conditions of the experiment. Credit: 2013 Y. Nagasaka et al.

Humans often synchronize their movements when, for example, we cooperate to move a piece of furniture. We also synchronize gestures and facial expressions when we interact. Coordinated actions are in fact surprisingly common in the animal kingdom, as exemplified by the flocking of birds and the schooling of fish. Such behaviors, however, have to date only been observed in the wild. Yasuo Nagasaka and colleagues from the Laboratory for Adaptive Intelligence at the RIKEN Brain Science Institute have now devised the first method for observing coordination under experimental conditions.

The researchers individually trained three Japanese to press two buttons repeatedly and alternately with one hand. They then recorded the monkeys performing this task with a video camera and device.

Nagasaka and his colleagues later paired the monkeys and had them perform the task again while facing each other (Fig. 1). Initially, each monkey in a pair pressed the buttons at different speeds. However, after a certain amount of time, the two monkeys spontaneously synchronized their button presses by altering the speed of their actions so that their button presses became harmonized with those of their partner.

The speed of repeated button presses differed among the three pairs of monkeys, as did the timing of the . In one pair, the button presses were synchronized but one monkey was always delayed by 1 millisecond, while in another the delay was 13 milliseconds. In all cases, however, the timing of the actions became closely matched, and the delay seemed to be dependent on exactly which monkeys had been paired together.

The researchers then played back the video recordings of the monkeys performing the task at different speeds while a monkey watched. The monkeys sped up or slowed down their button presses to harmonize their actions with those of the 'virtual' monkey, and they seemed to prefer to slow down their button presses, perhaps to save energy.

In a final set of experiments, the research team allowed the real to either see or hear the video recordings, and found that visual information is far more important than auditory information for synchronization.

"We believe that this spontaneous synchronization plays an important role in the building of social bonds, and we are now looking for the brain areas responsible," says Nagasaka. "This could be fundamental to understanding the brain itself, and also the social interaction deficits in conditions such as autism."

A video showing the spontaneous synchronization of monkey actions can be found here.

Explore further: Study of brain activity in monkeys shows how the brain processes mistakes made by others

More information: Nagasaka, Y. et al. Spontaneous synchronization of arm motion between Japanese macaques. Scientific Reports 3, 1151 (2013). dx.doi.org/10.1038/srep01151

Related Stories

Study of brain activity in monkeys shows how the brain processes mistakes made by others

January 11, 2013
Humans and other animals learn by making mistakes. They can also learn from observing the mistakes of others. The brain processes self-generated errors in a region called the medial frontal cortex (MFC) but little is known ...

Research shows reciprocity an important component of prosocial behavior

September 3, 2012
While exchanging favors with others, humans tend to think in terms of tit-for-tat, an assumption easily extended to other animals. As a result, reciprocity is often viewed as a cognitive feat requiring memory, perhaps even ...

Distinguishing yourself from others

April 22, 2011
(Medical Xpress) -- Researchers in Japan have identified the specific nerve cells responsible for the ability to distinguish between the actions of self and others. The discovery lays the foundations for studying social learning ...

Recommended for you

Nature or nurture? Innate social behaviors in the mouse brain

October 18, 2017
Adult male mice have a simple repertoire of innate, or instinctive, social behaviors: When encountering a female, a male mouse will try to mate with it, and when encountering another male, the mouse will attack. The animals ...

Brain activity predicts crowdfunding outcomes better than self-reports

October 18, 2017
Surveys and self-reports are a time-honored way of trying to predict consumer behavior, but they have limitations. People often give socially desirable answers or they simply don't know or remember things clearly.

Brain-machine interfaces to treat neurological disease

October 18, 2017
Since the 19th century at least, humans have wondered what could be accomplished by linking our brains – smart and flexible but prone to disease and disarray – directly to technology in all its cold, hard precision. Writers ...

Changing stroke definitions is causing chaos, warns professor

October 18, 2017
Proposals to change the definitions of stroke and related conditions are causing confusion and chaos in clinical practice and research, a Monash University associate professor has warned.

Navigational view of the brain thanks to powerful X-rays

October 18, 2017
If brain imaging could be compared to Google Earth, neuroscientists would already have a pretty good "satellite view" of the brain, and a great "street view" of neuron details. But navigating how the brain computes is arguably ...

'Wasabi receptor' for pain discovered in flatworms

October 18, 2017
A Northwestern University research team has discovered how scalding heat and tissue injury activate an ancient "pain" receptor in simple animals. The findings could lead to new strategies for analgesic drug design for the ...

0 comments

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.