How the brain encodes time and place

September 23, 2015
Oceans in the brain: How we remember different contexts
(Left) Many Ocean cells were active for only condition A or B (red and yellow). (Right) Island cells never were active for only one of the conditions. Credit: RIKEN

When you remember a particular experience, that memory has three critical elements—what, when, and where. MIT neuroscientists have now identified a brain circuit that processes the "when" and "where" components of memory.

This circuit, which connects the hippocampus and a region of the cortex known as entorhinal cortex, separates location and timing into two streams of information. The researchers also identified two populations of neurons in the entorhinal cortex that convey this information, dubbed "ocean cells" and "island cells."

Previous models of memory had suggested that the hippocampus, a brain structure critical for memory formation, separates timing and context information. However, the new study shows that this information is split even before it reaches the hippocampus.

"It suggests that there is a dichotomy of function upstream of the hippocampus," says Chen Sun, an MIT graduate student in brain and cognitive sciences and one of the lead authors of the paper, which appears in the Sept. 23 issue of Neuron. "There is one pathway that feeds temporal information into the hippocampus, and another that feeds contextual representations to the hippocampus."

The paper's other lead author is MIT postdoc Takashi Kitamura. The senior author is Susumu Tonegawa, the Picower Professor of Biology and Neuroscience and director of the RIKEN-MIT Center for Neural Circuit Genetics at MIT's Picower Institute for Learning and Memory. Other authors are Picower Institute technical assistant Jared Martin, Stanford University graduate student Lacey Kitch, and Mark Schnitzer, an associate professor of biology and applied physics at Stanford.

When and where

Located just outside the hippocampus, the entorhinal cortex relays sensory information from other cortical areas to the hippocampus, where memories are formed. Tonegawa and colleagues identified island and ocean cells a few years ago, and have been working since then to discover their functions.

In 2014, Tonegawa's lab reported that island cells, which form small clusters surrounded by ocean cells, are needed for the brain to form memories linking two events that occur in rapid succession. In the new Neuron study, the team found that ocean cells are required to create representations of a location where an event took place.

"Ocean cells are important for contextual representations," Sun says. "When you're in the library, when you're crossing the street, when you're on the subway, you have different memories associated with each of these contexts."

To discover these functions, the researchers labeled the two cell populations with a fluorescent molecule that lights up when it binds to calcium—an indication that the neuron is firing. This allowed them to determine which cells were active during tasks requiring mice to discriminate between two different environments, or to link two events in time.

The researchers also used a technique called optogenetics, which allows them to control neuron activity using light, to investigate how the mice's behavior changed when either island cells or ocean cells were silenced.

When they blocked ocean cell activity, the animals were no longer able to associate a certain environment with fear after receiving a foot shock there. Manipulating the island cells, meanwhile, allowed the researchers to lengthen or shorten the time gap between events that could be linked in the mice's memory.

Information flow

Previously, Tonegawa's lab found that the firing rates of island cells depend on how fast the animal is moving, leading the researchers to believe that island cells help the animal navigate their way through space. Ocean cells, meanwhile, help the animal to recognize where it is at a given time.

The researchers also found that these two streams of information flow from the entorhinal cortex to different parts of the hippocampus: Ocean cells send their contextual information to the CA3 and dentate gyrus regions, while island cells project to CA1 cells.

Tonegawa's lab is now pursuing further studies of how the entorhinal cortex and other parts of the brain represent time and place. The researchers are also investigating how information on timing and location are further processed in the brain to create a complete memory of an event.

"To form an episodic memory, each component has to be recombined together," Kitamura says. "This is the next question."

Explore further: Study reveals how the brain links memories of sequential events

More information: Kitamura T, Sun C, Martin J, Kitch LJ, Schnitzer MJ, and Tonegawa S. Entorhinal cortical Ocean cells encode specific contexts and drive context-specific fear memory. Neuron. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.08.036

Related Stories

Manipulating memory with light

October 9, 2014

Just look into the light: not quite, but researchers at the UC Davis Center for Neuroscience and Department of Psychology have used light to erase specific memories in mice, and proved a basic theory of how different parts ...

Brain circuits connected with memory discovered

November 7, 2011

(Medical Xpress) -- A new study published last week in Science reveals the discovery of a brain pathway that helps us link events that happen close together and play a role in memories.

Recommended for you

Chatter in the deep brain spurs empathy in rats

June 23, 2017

It's a classic conundrum: while rushing to get to an important meeting or appointment on time, you spot a stranger in distress. How do you decide whether to stop and help, or continue on your way?

The neural relationship between light and sleep

June 23, 2017

Humans are diurnal animals, meaning that we usually sleep at night and are awake during the day, due at least in part to light or the lack thereof. Light is known to affect sleep indirectly by entraining—modifying the length ...

How brains surrender to sleep

June 23, 2017

Scientists at the Research Institute of Molecular Pathology (IMP) in Vienna study fundamental aspects of sleep in roundworms. Using advanced technologies, they monitor the activity of all nerve cells in the brain while they ...

How pheromones trigger female sexual behavior

June 22, 2017

A study by a group of Japanese scientists showed how a male pheromone in mice enhances sexual behaviors in females—and how it may enhance a different behavior, aggression, in males—by identifying distinct neural circuits ...

3 comments

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

RobertKarlStonjek
3 / 5 (2) Sep 23, 2015
What of the 'place cells' discovered in rats. They are used to recall the maze layout.
RobertKarlStonjek
1 / 5 (1) Sep 24, 2015
"To form an episodic memory, each component has to be recombined together," Kitamura says. "This is the next question."


This is an incorrect assumption. The assumption is that memory is stored in the same way it is recalled. The probability favours memory not stored in this way. Evidence of memory anomalies, inaccuracies, 'tip-of-the-tongue' phenomena indicate reconstruction of memory stored in a form quite different to the recollected form.

Semantic memory is also capable of storing maps and histories, as when you read about a map or the history of a remote historically figure. Episodic memory features personal experience with less emphasis on the details that may change, semantic memory stores facts where personal experience of the knowledge is less important and often left out altogether.
Rustybolts
not rated yet Sep 24, 2015
I have this strange feeling I have read this article before.... ahh Deja Vu.

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.