How young children become moral and cooperative people

November 18, 2016 by Jane Kelly, University of Virginia
Vaish graduated from the College of Arts & Sciences in 2002 with a double major in psychology and English. Credit: University of Virginia

New work by a University of Virginia psychologist finds that the seeds of personal and social morality emerge as early as 5 years of age.

Amrisha Vaish, assistant professor of psychology, led the study, "Preschoolers Value Those Who Sanction Non-Cooperators," published in the journal Cognition. Specifically, the study found that 5-year-old recognize and value individuals who dissuade others from breaking moral norms.

Vaish and her team showed 4- and 5-year-olds two different videos. In the first, person A transgressed against person B, for example by breaking person B's toy. Person C observed the transgression and scolded the transgressor, person A, for her harmful behavior.

The second video featured the same scenario, with one important difference. Person C was replaced with person D, who did not scold person A for breaking the toy.

The 4- and 5-year-old children were then asked to respond to some questions about person C, the enforcer of the moral norm, and person D, the non-enforcer.

  • "Who did the right thing?"
  • "Whom would you prefer to play with?"
  • "Who do you not like so much?"

The children were also given cloth flowers to distribute between person C and person D in order to examine whether children reward enforcers with material resources.

"Five-year-olds, both with the questions we asked them and the flowers, were very clear in their preference for the enforcer and for giving more flowers to the enforcer rather than the non-enforcer," Vaish said.

"The 4-year-olds were much less clear – they did not clearly show a preference for, and did not seem to value, the enforcer more than the non-enforcer. They also did not hand out the flowers in a systematic way between the enforcer and non-enforcer," she said.

Good Guys vs. Bad Guys

Vaish's team honed in on testing 4- and 5-year-old children based on previous research. "Prior work, my own and some other researchers,' has shown that at 5 years, rather than 4, children begin to understand other sophisticated cooperative norms as well," she said. For instance, in a prior study, her team showed that 5-year-olds, but not 4-year-olds, expected transgressors to show guilt or remorse after breaking someone else's toy.

"We had the same kind of set up – in one video, a transgressor shows remorse, and in a different video, a different transgressor does not show remorse," she said. The children were asked similar questions and were given the flower task. "Five-year-olds very clearly prefer a transgressor who shows remorse; they think that that person has done the right thing, and they think that the non-remorseful transgressor is meaner and has not done the right thing.

"They also give more flowers to the person who has shown remorse," said Vaish, who said that 4-year-olds in the experiment didn't show any sort of systematic difference between the person who showed remorse and the one who did not.

Weighing Risk vs. Gain

But why cooperate in the first place? Vaish, whose research is rooted in questioning why human beings are predisposed to cooperating, said that question prompted the study.

"When I see a transgressor harming a victim, my intervening in that interaction is actually risky for me," she said. "The transgressor might turn around and actually retaliate against me. So I am putting myself at risk, which makes my intervention itself a cooperative act."

Vaish said cooperation in general is a puzzle. Why would we ever cooperate? Why don't we just look out for ourselves?

The psychologist offers a couple of reasons. "People likely cooperate for various reasons, both altruistic – because we genuinely care about the welfare of others – but also perhaps selfish reasons. For instance, cooperating earns me a good reputation, and I value my reputation; I want people to like me. And so if intervening in moral transgressions enhances my reputation, then I may be more likely to do so," she said.

The intervenor may also act because she or he is looking for some reciprocation in the future. And there may be material rewards at stake, much like the cloth flowers the children awarded in her studies.

"Prior research on the mechanisms of human cooperation has shown that adults value people who intervene in moral transgressions and the status of those who intervene or those who punish transgressors goes up; they receive more credit and they receive more resources," Vaish said. "We now see that that cooperative mechanism is operational surprisingly early in development."

Explore further: 'Sorry' doesn't heal children's hurt, but it mends relations

Related Stories

'Sorry' doesn't heal children's hurt, but it mends relations

November 11, 2015
Most adults know that a quick apology for a minor transgression, such as bumping into someone, helps maintain social harmony. The bumped-into person feels better, and so does the person who did the bumping. It's all part ...

Which discipline or reward is best for your child?

February 22, 2016
A new study lends insight into how children react to discipline practices used by parents and teachers, and sheds light on their view of what's fair.

Children also gossip

May 2, 2016
When it comes to selecting a cooperation partner, information about another person's reputation – for example as a generous person or a miser – may come handy. Many animal species make reputation judgements, but only ...

Children overeagerly seek social rules

September 27, 2016
Three-year-olds quickly absorb social norms. They even understand behaviors as rule-governed that are not subject to any norms, and insist that others adhere to these self-inferred "norms," a study by LMU psychologist Marco ...

How children perceive faces—seven-year-olds show different brain activities than adults

April 18, 2016
Humans are experts in facial perception. But not from birth. At which point do children become as skilled at it as adults?

Recommended for you

Intensive behavior therapy no better than conventional support in treating teenagers with antisocial behavior

January 19, 2018
Research led by UCL has found that intensive and costly multisystemic therapy is no better than conventional therapy in treating teenagers with moderate to severe antisocial behaviour.

Babies' babbling betters brains, language

January 18, 2018
Babies are adept at getting what they need - including an education. New research shows that babies organize mothers' verbal responses, which promotes more effective language instruction, and infant babbling is the key.

College branding makes beer more salient to underage students

January 18, 2018
In recent years, major beer companies have tried to capitalize on the salience of students' university affiliations, unveiling marketing campaigns and products—such as "fan cans," store displays, and billboard ads—that ...

Inherited IQ can increase in early childhood

January 18, 2018
When it comes to intelligence, environment and education matter – more than we think.

Baby brains help infants figure it out before they try it out

January 17, 2018
Babies often amaze their parents when they seemingly learn new skills overnight—how to walk, for example. But their brains were probably prepping for those tasks long before their first steps occurred, according to researchers.

Reducing sessions of trauma-focused psychotherapy does not affect effectiveness

January 17, 2018
Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) patients treated with as few as five sessions of trauma-focused psychotherapy find it equally effective as receiving 12 sessions.

0 comments

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.