Visual clues we use during walking and when we use them

July 25, 2017 by Bob Yirka report

(Medical Xpress)—A trio of researchers with the University of Texas and Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute has discovered which phase of visual information processing during human walking is used most to guide the feet accurately. In their paper published in Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, the group describes experiments they carried out with volunteers who walked a controlled course and what the researchers found by doing so.

Scientists have noted that despite the frequency of trips and falls, humans are very sophisticated walkers. We have to be, because walking on two legs leaves us constantly open to falling if we make a mistake. Prior research has shown that humans walk forward by lifting one foot, moving it forward, setting it back down onto the ground in front of us and then essentially falling onto it. Doing this repetitively leads to walking in a reasonably smooth fashion. But how do we make judgments and corrections about obstacles in time to keep from stumbling? In this new effort, the researchers enlisted the assistance of 44 volunteers between the ages of 18 and 22 and asked them to walk across a with illuminated targets for the volunteers to step on.

The researchers varied when the targets were illuminated and also how long they stayed visible, creating a variety of challenges for the volunteers. As the volunteers went through their paces, the researchers noted which challenges they faced and took measurements regarding how well they solved them, such as how much time transpired during each step.

The video will load shortly.
The critical control phase hypothesis. The color of the walker identifies the critical phase for visual control of the step to the corresponding target foothold. Credit: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (2017). doi: 10.1073/pnas.1611699114

The researchers found that the accuracy of the volunteers was greatest when they were able to make a stride adjustment during the last one and a half steps. In other words, rather than keeping track of all of the obstacles coming up, the brain mostly focused on just the few feet in front of the person, making corrections quickly on the fly. They also found that the brain did note upcoming obstacles—the volunteers did better when they could see two obstacles in sequence. They suggest that their results show that confer the ability to walk over complex terrain with efficiency.

The video will load shortly.
Visibility conditions for one of the experiments in the study. Filled circles indicate targets that are visible to the subject, while empty circles represent targets that are not visible. Credit: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (2017). doi: 10.1073/pnas.1611699114

Explore further: Study shows human brain pre-plays anticipated events in fast motion

More information: The critical phase for visual control of human walking over complex terrain, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (2017). DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1611699114 , http://www.pnas.org/content/early/2017/07/18/1611699114

Abstract
To walk efficiently over complex terrain, humans must use vision to tailor their gait to the upcoming ground surface without interfering with the exploitation of passive mechanical forces. We propose that walkers use visual information to initialize the mechanical state of the body before the beginning of each step so the resulting ballistic trajectory of the walker's center-of-mass will facilitate stepping on target footholds. Using a precision stepping task and synchronizing target visibility to the gait cycle, we empirically validated two predictions derived from this strategy: (1) Walkers must have information about upcoming footholds during the second half of the preceding step, and (2) foot placement is guided by information about the position of the target foothold relative to the preceding base of support. We conclude that active and passive modes of control work synergistically to allow walkers to negotiate complex terrain with efficiency, stability, and precision.

Related Stories

Study shows human brain pre-plays anticipated events in fast motion

May 24, 2017
(Medical Xpress)—A trio of researchers with Radboud University Nijmegen in the Netherlands has found evidence that suggests the human brain learns how some objects move and then replays it when it predicts a familiar scenario ...

Double-blind study suggests humans have olfactory defense against contagious disease

May 24, 2017
(Medical Xpress)—A European team of researchers working at Sweden's Karolinska Institutet has found evidence that suggests that humans have an olfactory defense against contagious diseases. In their paper published in Proceedings ...

Virtual reality 'out-of-body experience' reduces fear of death in volunteers

January 25, 2017
(Medical Xpress)—A team of researchers at Barcelona University has found that virtual reality experiences, if conducted in a certain way, can cause a reduced fear of dying. The team has written a paper describing their ...

Study shows when people feel anxious they are less reliable at reading emotions in other faces

May 31, 2017
(Medical Xpress)—A team of researchers with the MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit and the U.K. Centre for Tobacco and Alcohol Studies has found evidence of impaired emotional face reading by people when they are feeling ...

Brain scans reveal why we are more likely to take risks if we see others doing so

March 22, 2016
(Medical Xpress)—A combined team of researchers from the University of Melbourne and the California Institute of Technology has located a part of the brain involved in behavior contagion, by conducting a study involving ...

Recommended for you

Should I stay or should I leave? Untangling what goes on when a relationship is being questioned

August 17, 2017
Knowing whether to stay in or leave a romantic relationship is often an agonizing experience and that ambivalence can have negative consequences for health and well-being.

Kids learn moral lessons more effectively from stories with humans than human-like animals

August 17, 2017
A study by researchers at the Ontario Institute for Studies in Education (OISE) at the University of Toronto found that four to six-year-olds shared more after listening to books with human characters than books with anthropomorphic ...

History of stress increases miscarriage risk, says new review

August 17, 2017
A history of exposure to psychological stress can increase the risk of miscarriage by upto 42 per cent, according to a new review.

Study finds children pay close attention to potentially threatening information, avoid eye contact when anxious

August 17, 2017
We spend a lot of time looking at the eyes of others for social cues – it helps us understand a person's emotions, and make decisions about how to respond to them. We also know that adults avoid eye contact when anxious. ...

Communicating in a foreign language takes emotion out of decision making

August 16, 2017
If you could save the lives of five people by pushing another bystander in front of a train to his death, would you do it? And should it make any difference if that choice is presented in a language you speak, but isn't your ...

Precision medicine opens the door to scientific wellness preventive approaches to suicide

August 15, 2017
Researchers have developed a more precise way of diagnosing suicide risk, by developing blood tests that work in everybody, as well as more personalized blood tests for different subtypes of suicidality that they have newly ...

0 comments

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.