Why do we see similarities across languages? Human brain may be responsible

December 1, 2017
Credit: CC0 Public Domain

An estimated 7,099 languages are spoken throughout the world today. Almost a third of them are endangered—spoken by dwindling numbers—while just 23 languages represent more than half of the global population.

For years, researchers have been interested in the similarities seen across human languages. A new study led by University of Arizona researcher Masha Fedzechkina suggests that some of those similarities may be based on the human brain's preference for efficient information processing.

"If we look at languages of the world, they are very different on the surface, but they also share a lot of underlying commonalities, often called linguistic universals or cross-linguistic generalizations," said Fedzechkina, an assistant professor in the UA Department of Linguistics and lead author of the study, published in the journal Psychological Science.

"Most theories assume the reasons why languages have these cross-linguistic universals is because they're in some way constrained by the human brain," Fedzechkina said. "If these linguistic universals are indeed real, and if we understand their causes, then it can tell us something about how is acquired or processed by the human brain, which is one of the central questions in language sciences."

Fedzechkina and her collaborators conducted a study in which two groups of English-speaking-only individuals were each taught, over a three-day period, a different miniature artificial language designed by the experimenters. The two languages were structured differently from each other, and, importantly, neither was structured like the participants' native English.

In both groups, participants were taught two ways to express the same ideas. When later tested verbally—asked to describe actions in a video—participants, in the phrasing of their answers, showed an overwhelming preference for word orders that resulted in short "dependency length," which refers to the distance between words that depend on each other for interpretation.

The finding suggests that language universals might be explained, at least in part, by what appears to be the human brain's innate preference for "short dependencies."

"The longer the dependencies are, the harder they are to process in comprehension, presumably because of memory constraints," Fedzechkina said. "If we look cross-linguistically, we find that word orders of languages, overall, tend to have shorter dependencies than would be expected by chance, suggesting that there is a correlation between constraints on human information processing and the structures of natural languages. We wanted to do this research to provide the first behavioral evidence for a causal link between the two, and we did. We found that when learners have two options in the input grammar, they tend to prefer the option that reduces dependency lengths and thus makes sentences in the language easier for the to process."

What Artificial Languages Can Teach Us

The researchers' decision to use artificial languages for their study was strategic.

"Traditionally, linguists have studied cross-linguistic universals by going to different cultures and documenting the structures of different languages, and then they looked for cross-linguistic commonalities," Fedzechkina said. "That research has been transformative in identifying a large number of potential linguistic universals and has generated a lot of theories about why these universals exist, but it also has its drawbacks."

Among those drawbacks: It has been challenging to tease out the role of the brain.

"If you look, for example, at languages like Spanish and Italian, they share a lot of structural commonalities, but many of these commonalities are there because both languages originated from Latin," Fedzechkina said. "Also, languages that are related to each other geographically often share structures, too—for example, due to population movement—even if they are not related historically. Once we take into account these historical and geographic dependencies, we might not have enough independent data points to convincingly test hypotheses about language universals."

By teaching naïve study participants an artificial language that has certain structures that are not present in their native language, and then looking at what kind of structures they prefer after they have learned these languages, researchers can draw inferences about the causality underlying language universals, Fedzechkina said.

"If the pattern is not present in the input and if it's not present in the participants' native language, but they still introduce this pattern, it likely reveals more general cognitive biases humans have," she said.

The finding gives researchers a better understanding of the role of human cognition in language and acquisition.

"We know from work on second language acquisition that learners' native language influences the way they learn a second or third language, and the fact that our participants relied so strongly on the deeper underlying principle of human information processing rather than on surface word order of their was very surprising and very impressive for us," Fedzechkina said. "We provide the first behavioral evidence for the hypothesized connection between human information processing and language structure, and we suggest that processing constraints do play a role in language acquisition, and the way language changes over time."

Explore further: If your child is bilingual, learning additional languages later might be easier

More information: Maryia Fedzechkina et al, Human Information Processing Shapes Language Change, Psychological Science (2017). DOI: 10.1177/0956797617728726

Related Stories

If your child is bilingual, learning additional languages later might be easier

October 2, 2017
It is often claimed that people who are bilingual are better than monolinguals at learning languages. Now, the first study to examine bilingual and monolingual brains as they learn an additional language offers new evidence ...

Language structure arises from balance of clear and effective communication

October 15, 2012
When learning a new language, we automatically organize words into sentences that will be both clearly understood and efficient (quick) to communicate. That's the finding of a new study reported today in the Proceedings of ...

Our brains are hardwired for language

April 17, 2014
A groundbreaking study published in PLOS ONE by Prof. Iris Berent of Northeastern University and researchers at Harvard Medical School shows the brains of individual speakers are sensitive to language universals. Syllables ...

Using fMRI and multivoxel pattern analysis reveals independent brain activation patterns in bilingual people

July 13, 2017
(Medical Xpress)—A team of researchers from China and the U.S. has found independent brain activation patterns in bilingual people when they switch between languages. In their paper published on the open access site Science ...

Recommended for you

Suicidal thoughts rapidly reduced with ketamine, finds study

December 14, 2017
Ketamine was significantly more effective than a commonly used sedative in reducing suicidal thoughts in depressed patients, according to researchers at Columbia University Medical Center (CUMC). They also found that ketamine's ...

Do bullies have more sex?

December 14, 2017
Adolescents who are willing to exploit others for personal gain are more likely to bully and have sex than those who score higher on a measure of honesty and humility. This is according to a study in Springer's journal Evolutionary ...

Children's screen-time guidelines too restrictive, according to new research

December 14, 2017
Digital screen use is a staple of contemporary life for adults and children, whether they are browsing on laptops and smartphones, or watching TV. Paediatricians and scientists have long expressed concerns about the impact ...

Eating together as a family helps children feel better, physically and mentally

December 14, 2017
Children who routinely eat their meals together with their family are more likely to experience long-term physical and mental health benefits, a new Canadian study shows.

The iceberg model of self-harm

December 14, 2017
Researchers have created a model of self-harm that shows high levels of the problem in the community, especially in young girls, and the need for school-based prevention measures.

Anti-stress compound reduces obesity and diabetes

December 13, 2017
For the first time, scientists from the Max Planck Institute of Psychiatry in Munich could prove that a stress protein found in muscle has a diabetes promoting effect. This finding could pave the way to a completely new treatment ...

3 comments

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

dnatwork
not rated yet Dec 01, 2017
To be valid, it would have to be tested with speakers of languages other than English. Do they prefer the same dependency lengths? If not, then these researchers have only found something about people who speak English, and they are back to the problem they started with.
plutoid
not rated yet Dec 02, 2017
check out Zipfs law on you tube. the Zipf graph applies to much more than language and is a curious theory about the structure of the brain.
HenryE
not rated yet Dec 02, 2017
Many basic sounds and utterances may have been passed down since the earliest vocalizations humans made, and it isn't surprising they have persisted in languages as we spread around the globe.

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.