How we explain the behavior of others depends on our beliefs about their 'true selves'

August 14, 2018 by Denise Valenti, Princeton University
Credit: CC0 Public Domain

Why did they do that? It's a question we ask every day in attempting to understand the behavior of others and make meaning of the world around us. How we answer the question, however, varies depending on our moral attitudes toward the behavior.

In a paper appearing in the November 2018 issue of Cognition, Simon Cullen, a postdoctoral research associate in the Princeton Neuroscience Institute, found that people who disapprove of a are more likely to explain it in terms of the situation or circumstances rather than attributing it to the other person's agency.

According to Cullen, this reflects a bias to believe that people have "virtuous true selves"—the idea that, at least "deep down," everyone is innately good, so their true self couldn't possibly be in accord with what is deemed a negative behavior.

"We have really surprisingly strong prior beliefs about people's essences," Cullen said. "We are not aware we have these prior beliefs, and we don't tend to examine them, but they deeply influence how we explain people's actions, and how I think we react to the morally significant ones."

Cullen's paper, "When do circumstances excuse? Moral prejudices and beliefs about the true self drive preferences for agency-minimizing explanations," builds from his work as a doctoral student in Princeton's Department of Philosophy. He earned his Ph.D. in 2015.

Approximately 2,500 adults participated in multiple studies conducted by Cullen. In each study, participants were asked to explain a different morally significant action.

In one study, for example, participants read about a young woman who decides to have an abortion. Participants rated various potential explanations for her decision. Some of these explanations emphasized the role of the woman's own agency, while others emphasized the role of her circumstances. Cullen found that the more strongly participants condemned abortion, the more likely they became to report that some external factor (for example, peer pressure) must have caused the individual to make that choice.

The findings were perhaps even more incisive in a study of people's reactions to someone who crosses political lines, Cullen said. He presented to participants a scenario where a person from a liberal family became a political conservative and vice-versa. For comparison, he presented an alternate scenario where a person remains true to their family's political views.

Cullen found that when explaining a person's decision to defect from their parents' party, conservative participants were far more strongly inclined to explain the decision in terms of the person's values if the person defected from a liberal family rather than a conservative one. When participants explained political conformism within a family, both liberals and conservatives were more likely to explain a person's political identity in terms of their upbringing if they were a member of the outgroup rather than the ingroup.

Cullen explained this as a function of conservatives' tendency to value ingroup loyalty and respect for authority, making defection from one's family politics a more serious moral violation.

The effect of assigning a virtuous true self to another can be alienating and place distance between people, Cullen said. "It's a way to confuse how you believe the world should be with how the world is," he said. "That's usually a bad thing to do. It's much better to figure out how the world is."

Especially in today's polarized political environment, the belief in a virtuous true self makes broaching discussion, let alone reaching any sort of middle ground, next to impossible, he said. "It makes me so much less open to you if I start off with my beliefs about your most essential self," he said.

Explore further: Red or blue in the face? Study says conservatives show less emotion

More information: Simon Cullen. When do circumstances excuse? Moral prejudices and beliefs about the true self drive preferences for agency-minimizing explanations, Cognition (2018). DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.06.021

Related Stories

Red or blue in the face? Study says conservatives show less emotion

April 3, 2018
If playing in a high-stakes poker game, it may be better to face opponents on the liberal side of the political spectrum.

Who is the fairest of them all? Research reveals we hold a rose-tinted view of our own morality

November 24, 2016
Recent research published in the journal Social Psychological and Personality Science has shown that when evaluating their own morality, people will almost always judge themselves as morally superior to the average person.

Recommended for you

Even toddlers weigh risks, rewards when making choices

September 21, 2018
Every day, adults conduct cost-benefit analyses in some form for decisions large and small, economic and personal: Bring a lunch or go out? Buy or rent? Remain single or start a family? All are balances of risk and reward.

Early warning sign of psychosis detected

September 21, 2018
Brains of people at risk of psychosis exhibit a pattern that can help predict whether they will go on to develop full-fledged schizophrenia, a new Yale-led study shows. The findings could help doctors begin early intervention ...

Quitting junk food produces similar withdrawal-type symptoms as drug addiction

September 20, 2018
If you plan to try and quit junk food, expect to suffer similar withdrawal-type symptoms—at least during the initial week—like addicts experience when they attempt to quit using drugs.

In depression the brain region for stress control is larger

September 20, 2018
Although depression is one of the leading psychiatric disorders in Germany, its cause remains unclear. A recent study at the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences (MPI CBS) in Leipzig, Germany, found ...

American girls read and write better than boys

September 20, 2018
As early as the fourth grade, girls perform better than boys on standardized tests in reading and writing, and as they get older that achievement gap widens even more, according to research published by the American Psychological ...

Mindfulness meditation: 10 minutes a day improves cognitive function

September 19, 2018
Practising mindfulness meditation for 10 minutes a day improves concentration and the ability to keep information active in one's mind, a function known as "working memory". The brain achieves this by becoming more efficient, ...

2 comments

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

Nik_2213
1 / 5 (1) Aug 14, 2018
Same as any computer program or simulation. Never mind the algorithm, GIGO...
MuddShark
not rated yet Aug 16, 2018
As I read this at 11:10 p.m., I'm compelled to ponder the essential self of the person across the street ad libbing a rap "song" at extreme volume and liberal locution of "fuck" and "nigga".

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.