This article has been reviewed according to Science X's editorial process and policies. Editors have highlighted the following attributes while ensuring the content's credibility:

fact-checked

peer-reviewed publication

trusted source

proofread

Researchers discover neuronal mechanism linked to a minutes-long decision process in fruit flies

Researchers discover neuronal mechanism linked to a minutes-long decision process in fruit flies
oviDN [Ca2+] dips during ovulation, rises for seconds to minutes and peaks immediately before the abdomen bend for egg deposition. a, Behavioral sequence of egg laying. b, Egg expressing GCaMP3 in the body. Steps correspond to a. Insets show close-ups, with over/undersaturated pixels in red/blue; main panels show over/undersaturated pixels in white/black. c, Behavioral progression. Lines connect single egg-laying sequences. d, Schematic of wheel. e, Single oviDNb traced from light microscopy images. Blue arrow indicates soma in brain, green arrow indicates outputs in the abdominal ganglion. f, oviDN somas on the right side of the brain labeled by oviDN-SS1. g, oviDN ∆F/F and behavior during laying of two eggs by the same fly. ∆F/F is smoothed with a 2 s boxcar filter. Images are z-projection of selected imaging slices, with labels referring to oviDNa and oviDNb (oviDNa is partially obscured by oviDNb). h, Population-averaged oviDNb ∆F/F aligned to the end of the abdomen bend for egg laying. Light gray shading represents ±s.e.m. throughout; 43 imaging traces from 41 egg-laying events associated with nine cells in eight flies. The number of traces exceeds the number of egg-laying events because for two eggs we imaged oviDNb on both sides of the brain. Behavioral events shown below. i, Schematic of abdomen bend. θ denotes ‘body angle’ and length is neck–ovipositor distance. jl, Mean oviDN ∆F/F and behavior aligned to events in h: ‘ovulation start’ (j), ‘search start’ (k) and completion of abdomen bend (l). ‘Normalized length’ is the length given in i divided by its median (Methods). Shorter, thicker arrows indicate when abdomen bend for egg deposition is complete. A subsequent (stronger) bend is, presumably, for cleaning the ovipositor. m, oviDN ∆F/F during individual egg-laying events, smoothed with a 5 s boxcar filter. Black line, mean. n, Mean oviDN ∆F/F during egg laying for all seven flies that laid three or more eggs, smoothed with a 5 s boxcar filter. A single GCaMP7b fly is shown in gray. NP, Nippon Project; Ave., average; 2-p, two-photon; Ephys, electrophysiology; Max., maximum. Credit: Nature (2023). DOI: 10.1038/s41586-023-06271-6

Parents famously go to great lengths to give their kids the best start in life—the right school district, piano lessons, orthodontics. Fruit flies are no different, even if setting their progeny up for success is more basic: If you're a fly, finding the right spot to lay your eggs—one where they'll be safe from spiders and starvation—is one of the most important life decisions you will ever make.

Now, a new study published in Nature has shed light on how mull their options while deciding where to lay their eggs—findings that could build a foundation for understanding how humans make educated and .

"We identified a rise-to-threshold calcium signal in the that ultimately guides a choice between several options, in this case, where to lay an egg," says first author Vikram Vijayan, a postdoc in Rockefeller's Laboratory of Integrated Brain Function headed by Gaby Maimon. "It's an important step towards understanding, generally, how information processing in the brain translates into discrete actions."

Decision time

All decisions are not created equal. Some are easily made in the blink of an eye—if a rock comes whizzing at your face, you'll duck without much thought. But others, such as ordering an entree at a restaurant, are informed by weighing different options and might take several minutes.

Such is the case for flies pondering where to leave their eggs. To study that, Vijayan built a little, rotatable, treadmill that allowed head-fixed flies to walk across different surfaces with varying sucrose concentrations, mimicking different parts of a fruit that they might encounter in the real world. Since the flies were walking in place, the researchers could measure as the insects perused the options.

Meanwhile, the researchers homed in on a specific set of brain cells known as oviposition descending neurons (oviDNs), which, in prior work, had been generally linked to the ability to lay eggs.

Credit: Rockefeller University

The researchers' sought to understand whether activity in these cells might inform when and where flies lay eggs. Prior work in neuroscience had shown that decision-making-related cells express activity that rises to a threshold level when a decision is taken. However, these past signals had only been shown to rise over a few seconds or less, meaning that they could explain how a person ducks when a rock comes whizzing at them but not how someone selects their dinner at a restaurant.

By imaging the activity of oviDNs, the researchers discovered a calcium signal that fluctuated up and down as flies inspected different egg-laying options, eventually reaching threshold or peak level exactly at the moment egg-laying was initiated. Advancing on past work here, the rising process could take a minute or more to reach threshold, providing a mechanism for decisions on this longer timescale.

Initially, the researchers simply observed the neuronal process; but later, they carefully began adjusting the process to see if it would affect the decisions taken. At times, Vijayan activated the neurons to expedite the rise-to-threshold process. When he did so, he noticed that the eggs emerged as soon as the neuronal activity reached the threshold value, confirming that the egg-laying decision was indeed linked to this process hitting its threshold.

"Generally speaking, it has been hard to causally tie threshold crossing with action initiation in any animal," Vijayan says. "But in this case, we found that the rise-to-threshold process causes an action to happen."

At other times, Vijayan gently inhibited the oviDNs, prolonging the time it took for the process they participate in to reach threshold. Flies in those experiments took longer to make a decision and when they did, the flies also made more accurate decisions. That is, they laid even more of their eggs on the substrate that matched their expected preference in the wild.

This finding suggested something humans already suspect—often, the more we are willing to weigh options, the more likely we are to pick a better one and thus make better decisions. "The same is true for flies. The more time the flies spent exploring, the more they tended to pick an option that presumably ensures better survival of their offspring," Vijayan says.

New avenues

The researchers believe that the findings from this study may ultimately inform how decision-making processes unfold in mammals and perhaps even humans.

"This work allows us to imagine that a similar rise-to-threshold process might exist in our own brain as we pick clothes to wear in the morning––a task that resembles the one our flies are performing," says Maimon. He also notes that if we can understand how minutes-long decision processes work in the healthy brain, this can only help to inform, down the road, approaches for treating neuropsychiatric diseases where decision-making abilities are altered on this timescale.

As next steps, Maimon and Vijayan want to understand the molecular mechanisms that enable rising signals for decision making to be built. "The calcium signal we see in these neurons sometimes moves up and down for reasons we can't explain. We explored one factor—the relative chemical composition of the options—to explain the rise and fall of the calcium signal. Exploring other factors might help us to understand more comprehensively how this process unfolds and thus how minutes-long decisions are taken by animals in nature," Maimon says.

More information: Gaby Maimon, A rise-to-threshold process for a relative value decision, Nature (2023). DOI: 10.1038/s41586-023-06271-6. www.nature.com/articles/s41586-023-06271-6

Journal information: Nature
Citation: Researchers discover neuronal mechanism linked to a minutes-long decision process in fruit flies (2023, July 5) retrieved 27 April 2024 from https://medicalxpress.com/news/2023-07-neuronal-mechanism-linked-minutes-long-decision.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further

How seeing corpses reduces the lifespan of flies

55 shares

Feedback to editors