This article has been reviewed according to Science X's editorial process and policies. Editors have highlighted the following attributes while ensuring the content's credibility:

fact-checked

peer-reviewed publication

trusted source

proofread

Suppressing negative thoughts may be good for mental health after all, study suggests

negative thoughts
Credit: Pixabay/CC0 Public Domain

The commonly-held belief that attempting to suppress negative thoughts is bad for our mental health could be wrong, a new study from scientists at the University of Cambridge suggests.

Researchers at the Medical Research Council (MRC) Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit trained 120 volunteers worldwide to suppress thoughts about negative events that worried them, and found that not only did these become less vivid, but that the participants' mental health also improved.

"We're all familiar with the Freudian idea that if we suppress our feelings or thoughts, then these thoughts remain in our unconscious, influencing our behavior and well-being perniciously," said Professor Michael Anderson.

"The whole point of psychotherapy is to dredge up these thoughts so one can deal with them and rob them of their power. In more recent years, we've been told that suppressing thoughts is intrinsically ineffective and that it actually causes people to think the thought more—it's the classic idea of 'Don't think about a pink elephant.'"

These ideas have become dogma in the clinical treatment realm, said Anderson, with national guidelines talking about thought avoidance as a major maladaptive coping behavior to be eliminated and overcome in depression, anxiety, PTSD, for example.

When COVID-19 appeared in 2020, like many researchers, Professor Anderson wanted to see how his own research could be used to help people through the pandemic. His interest lay in a brain mechanism known as inhibitory control—the ability to override our reflexive responses—and how it might be applied to memory retrieval, and in particular to stopping the retrieval of negative thoughts when confronted with potent reminders to them.

Dr. Zulkayda Mamat—at the time a Ph.D. student in Professor Anderson's lab and at Trinity College, Cambridge—believed that inhibitory control was critical in overcoming trauma in experiences occurring to herself and many others she has encountered in life. She had wanted to investigate whether this was an innate ability or something that was learnt—and hence could be taught.

Dr. Mamat said, "Because of the pandemic, we were seeing a need in the community to help people cope with surging anxiety. There was already a , a hidden epidemic of mental health problems, and this was getting worse. So with that backdrop, we decided to see if we could help people cope better."

Professor Anderson and Dr. Mamat recruited 120 people across 16 countries to test whether it might in fact be possible—and beneficial—for people to practice suppressing their fearful thoughts. Their findings are published in Science Advances.

In the study, each participant was asked to think of a number of scenarios that might plausibly occur in their lives over the next two years—20 negative "fears and worries" that they were afraid might happen, 20 positive "hopes and dreams," and 36 routine and mundane neutral events. The fears had to be worries of current concern to them, that have repeatedly intruded in their thoughts.

Each event had to be specific to them and something they had vividly imagined occurring. For each scenario, they were to provide a cue word (an obvious reminder that could be used to evoke the event during training) and a key detail (a single word expressing a central event detail). For example:

  • Negative—visiting one's parents at the hospital as a result of COVID-19, with the cue "Hospital" and the detail "Breathing."
  • Neutral—a visit to the opticians, with the cue "Optician" and the detail "Cambridge."
  • Positive—seeing one's sister get married, with the cue "Wedding" and the detail "Dress."

Participants were asked to rate each event on a number of points: vividness, likelihood of occurrence, distance in the future, level of anxiety about the event (or level of joy for positive events), frequency of , degree of current concern, long-term impact, and .

Participants also completed questionnaires to assess their mental health, though no one was excluded, allowing the researchers to look at a broad range of participants, including many with serious depression, anxiety, and pandemic-related post-traumatic stress.

Then, over Zoom, Dr. Mamat took each participant through the 20-minute training, which involved 12 "No-imagine" and 12 "Imagine" repetitions for events, each day for three days.

For No-imagine trials, participants were given one of their cue words, asked to first acknowledge the event in their mind. Then, while continuing to stare directly at the reminder cue, they were asked to stop thinking about the event—they should not try to imagine the event itself or use diversionary thoughts to distract themselves, but rather should try to block any images or thoughts that the reminder might evoke. For this part of the trial, one group of participants was given their to suppress and the other given their neutral ones.

For Imagine trials, participants were given a cue word and asked to imagine the event as vividly as possible, thinking what it would be like and imagining how they would feel at the event. For ethical reasons, no participant was given a negative event to imagine, but only positive or neutral ones.

At the end of the third day and again three months later, participants were once again asked to rate each event on vividness, level of anxiety, emotional intensity, etc., and completed questionnaires to assess changes in depression, anxiety, worry, affect, and well-being, key facets of mental health.

Dr. Mamat said, "It was very clear that those events that participants practiced suppressing were less vivid, less emotionally anxiety-inducing, than the other events and that overall, participants improved in terms of their mental health. But we saw the biggest effect among those participants who were given practice at suppressing fearful, rather than neutral, thoughts."

Following training—both immediately and after three months—participants reported that suppressed events were less vivid and less fearful. They also found themselves thinking about these events less.

Suppressing thoughts even improved mental health among participants with likely post-traumatic stress disorder. Among participants with post-traumatic stress who suppressed negative thoughts, their negative mental health indices scores fell on average by 16% (compared to a 5% fall for similar participants suppressing neutral events), whereas positive mental health indices scores increased by almost 10% (compared to a 1% fall in the second group).

In general, people with worse symptoms at the outset of the study improved more after suppression training, but only if they suppressed their fears. This finding directly contradicts the notion that suppression is a maladaptive coping process.

Suppressing negative thoughts did not lead to a "rebound," where a participant recalled these events more vividly. Only one person out of 120 showed higher detail recall for suppressed items post-training, and just six of the 61 participants that suppressed fears reported increased vividness for No-Imagine items post-training, but this was in line with the baseline rate of vividness increases that occurred for events that were not suppressed at all.

"What we found runs counter to the accepted narrative," said Professor Anderson. "Although more work will be needed to confirm the findings, it seems like it is possible and could even be potentially beneficial to actively suppress our fearful thoughts."

Although participants were not asked to continue practicing the technique, many of them chose to do so spontaneously. When Dr. Mamat contacted the participants after three months, she found that the benefits in terms of reduced levels of depression and negative emotions, continued for all participants, but were most pronounced among those participants who continued to use the technique in their daily lives.

"The follow up was my favorite time of my entire Ph.D., because every day was just joyful," she said. "I didn't have a single participant who told me 'Oh, I feel bad' or 'This was useless.' I didn't prompt them or ask 'Did you find this helpful?' They were just automatically telling me how helpful they found it."

One participant was so impressed by the technique that she taught her daughter and her own mother how to do it. Another reported how she had moved home just prior to COVID-19 and so felt very isolated during the pandemic.

"She said this study had come exactly at the time she needed it because she was having all these , all these worries and anxiety about the future, and this really, really helped her," said Dr. Mamat.

More information: Zulkayda Mamat et al, Improving Mental Health by Training the Suppression of Unwanted Thoughts, Science Advances (2023). DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.adh5292. www.science.org/doi/10.1126/sciadv.adh5292

Journal information: Science Advances
Citation: Suppressing negative thoughts may be good for mental health after all, study suggests (2023, September 20) retrieved 27 April 2024 from https://medicalxpress.com/news/2023-09-suppressing-negative-thoughts-good-mental.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further

Study reveals role of sleep deprivation in unwanted thoughts

418 shares

Feedback to editors