This article has been reviewed according to Science X's editorial process and policies. Editors have highlighted the following attributes while ensuring the content's credibility:

fact-checked

peer-reviewed publication

trusted source

proofread

New study reveals the signals in your brain that initiate spontaneous actions

diving board
Credit: Unsplash/CC0 Public Domain

A new study published in Nature Communications examines how the brain initiates spontaneous actions. In addition to demonstrating how spontaneous action emerges without environmental input, this research has implications for the origins of slow ramping of neural activity before movement onset—a commonly-observed but poorly understood phenomenon.

The study was led by Jake Gavenas, Ph.D., while he was a Ph.D. student at the Brain Institute at Chapman University, and co-authored by two faculty members of the Brain Institute, Uri Maoz and Aaron Schurger. In their work, the researchers propose an answer to that question. They simulated spontaneous activity in simple neural networks and compared this simulated activity to intracortical recordings of humans when they moved spontaneously.

The study results suggest something striking: Many rapidly fluctuating neurons can interact in a network to give rise to very slow fluctuations at the level of the neuron population.

Imagine, for example, standing atop a high-dive platform and trying to summon the willpower to jump. Nothing in the outside world tells you when to jump; that decision comes from within. At some point you experience deciding to jump and then you jump.

In the background, your brain (or more specifically, your motor cortex) sends that cause carefully coordinated muscle contractions across your body, resulting in you running and jumping. But where in the brain do these signals originate, and how do they relate to the of willing your body to move?

Starting from the 1960s, neuroscientists have found that electrical activity in the brain ramps up one to two seconds before spontaneous voluntary actions. Many scientists thought that the onset of this ramping reflected preparation to move following a preconscious decision to act. But, despite investigations into the origins of this slowly ramping activity, it seemed as though it suddenly emerged "out of nowhere."

In the years since, neuroscientists and philosophers have debated what this ramping means for free will and conscious self-control. If, following an early decision to move, the brain is preparing to move two seconds (or more, according to some studies) before you consciously decide to move, might your actions be largely unconsciously predetermined? Understanding the neural origin of this ramping activity is therefore a paramount problem in neuroscience.

The view that there is preconscious information in the brain seconds before action onset has been challenged by neuroscientists like Maoz and Schurger.

In particular, in 2012, Schurger suggested that the slow ramping brain activity is part of a larger process, in which slow background fluctuations in activity must reach a certain threshold to initiate movement.

If those slow fluctuations help to determine the moment of threshold crossing, then looking back from movement onset ensures that you will observe slow ramping beforehand, even if the ramping is not the outcome of an early preconscious decision to move. In this view, the important event is not the onset of the slow-ramping process but instead the crossing of the threshold.

Although compelling, this explanation leaves a key unanswered question: where do these slow background fluctuations in neural activity--commonly known as 1/f noise--come from in the first place, given that the activity of individual neurons fluctuates quite rapidly?

This study is the first to explain how those slow background fluctuations emerge from networks of neurons, where none of the individual neurons by themselves operate on that long of a time-scale. Those slow fluctuations may then, in turn, contribute to a threshold-crossing event, of the kind that is thought to trigger movement, and thus to the slow ramping that is evident before spontaneous action onset and beyond.

Gavenas said, "We see similar slow-ramping signals before other kinds of spontaneous behaviors, like coming up with creative ideas or freely remembering things that have happened to you. A similar process might therefore underlie those phenomena, but only time and further research will tell."

In sum, this is a landmark study because it offers a potential explanation for the origin of slow, spontaneous fluctuations in population-level neural activity, which is a ubiquitous phenomenon in neural systems.

In addition, according to Maoz, "It reveals the bias we have as researchers to think that our results uncover a causal mechanism, when it may really be just a correlation."

More information: Jake Gavenas et al, Slow ramping emerges from spontaneous fluctuations in spiking neural networks, Nature Communications (2024). DOI: 10.1038/s41467-024-51401-x

Journal information: Nature Communications
Provided by Chapman University
Citation: New study reveals the signals in your brain that initiate spontaneous actions (2024, September 5) retrieved 5 September 2024 from https://medicalxpress.com/news/2024-09-reveals-brain-spontaneous-actions.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further

Chaotic dynamics in the brain may enable probabilistic thinking

9 shares

Feedback to editors