Action videogames change brains: study

April 26, 2012, University of Toronto

A team led by psychology professor Ian Spence at the University of Toronto reveals that playing an action videogame, even for a relatively short time, causes differences in brain activity and improvements in visual attention.

Previous studies have found differences in between action videogame players and non-players, but these could have been attributed to pre-existing differences in the brains of those predisposed to playing videogames and those who avoid them. This is the first time research has attributed these differences directly to .

Twenty-five subjects — who had not previously played videogames — played a game for a total of 10 hours in one to two hour sessions. Sixteen of the subjects played a first-person shooter game and, as a control, nine subjects played a three-dimensional puzzle game.

Before and after playing the games, the subjects' brain waves were recorded while they tried to detect a target object among other distractions over a wide visual field. Subjects who played the shooter videogame and also showed the greatest improvement on the task showed significant changes in their brain waves. The remaining subjects — including those who had played the puzzle game — did not.

"After playing the shooter game, the changes in electrical activity were consistent with brain processes that enhance visual attention and suppress distracting information," said Sijing Wu, a PhD student in Spence's lab in U of T's Department of Psychology and lead author of the study.

"Studies in different labs, including here at the University of Toronto, have shown that action can improve selective visual attention, such as the ability to quickly detect and identify a target in a cluttered background," said Spence. "But nobody has previously demonstrated that there are differences in activity which are a direct result of playing the videogame."

"Superior visual attention is crucial in many important everyday activities," added Spence. "It's necessary for things such as driving a car, monitoring changes on a computer display, or even avoiding tripping while walking through a room with children's toys scattered on the floor."

The research was supported by funding from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada in the form of Discovery Grants to Spence and co-author Claude Alain of the Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Centre and U of T's Psychology Department.

The research team also included U of T PhD candidate Cho Kin Cheng, Jing Feng, a postdoc at the Rotman Research Institute, and former undergraduate student Lisa D'Angelo.

More information: The study will appear in the June 2012 issue of the Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, published by MIT Press. Early access to uncorrected proofs of the article is available now at: www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/a … 10.1162/jocn_a_00192

Related Stories

Recommended for you

Research reveals atomic-level changes in ALS-linked protein

January 18, 2018
For the first time, researchers have described atom-by-atom changes in a family of proteins linked to amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS), a group of brain disorders known as frontotemporal dementia and degenerative diseases ...

Fragile X finding shows normal neurons that interact poorly

January 18, 2018
Neurons in mice afflicted with the genetic defect that causes Fragile X syndrome (FXS) appear similar to those in healthy mice, but these neurons fail to interact normally, resulting in the long-known cognitive impairments, ...

How your brain remembers what you had for dinner last night

January 17, 2018
Confirming earlier computational models, researchers at University of California San Diego and UC San Diego School of Medicine, with colleagues in Arizona and Louisiana, report that episodic memories are encoded in the hippocampus ...

Recording a thought's fleeting trip through the brain

January 17, 2018
University of California, Berkeley neuroscientists have tracked the progress of a thought through the brain, showing clearly how the prefrontal cortex at the front of the brain coordinates activity to help us act in response ...

Midbrain 'start neurons' control whether we walk or run

January 17, 2018
Locomotion comprises the most fundamental movements we perform. It is a complex sequence from initiating the first step, to stopping when we reach our goal. At the same time, locomotion is executed at different speeds to ...

Miles Davis is not Mozart: The brains of jazz and classical pianists work differently

January 16, 2018
Keith Jarret, world-famous jazz pianist, once answered in an interview when asked if he would ever be interested in doing a concert where he would play both jazz and classical music: "No, that's hilarious. [...] It's like ...

5 comments

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

baudrunner
3 / 5 (2) Apr 26, 2012
A more sophisticated study in the U.K. involved testing the IQ's of people from various disciplines and who were employed full-time at their jobs. The study included workers from different occupations - teachers, policeman, laborers, engineers, nurses - the whole gamut. The IQ's of the study group who had remained employed for the three or so years of the study were compared to their scores as tested before the study began. It was found that taxi drivers experienced by far the greatest increase in IQ, as many as five points, among those who had registered higher scores. The inference is obvious, keep the brain active and working to prevent deterioration, and possibly dementia, in old age.
antialias_physorg
not rated yet Apr 26, 2012
Might also have to do with the fact that our heritage rewarded quick adaptation to visual clues (or get eaten) while puzzle solving skills are more of a long term strategy and therefore would not merit a fast neural adaptation as a survival advantage.
Sean_W
1 / 5 (1) Apr 26, 2012
I am not surprised at the results but this has one of the key weaknesses of the same junk science people us to condemn video games: low sample size. Someone should do a larger study and don't use meta-analysis so the results can be more credible.
Judgeking
1 / 5 (1) Apr 27, 2012
What surprises me about this study is that they found 25 people 'who had not previously played videogames'!

Seriously though, anyone who plays FPSs regularly knows that the games improve your real life reflexes/reaction time.
neurosynchrony
not rated yet May 03, 2012
What the authors failed to measure was prefrontal cortex activity. A colleague of mine has shown that first person shooter games shut the PFC down due to illiciting constant fight-or-flight responses based on anticipation of an enemy. While their visual systems are more tuned, they can no longer concentrate. Not the best trade off.

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.