Deconstructing motor skills: Separate aspects of development highlighted in study

September 30, 2013 by Peter Reuell
With the help of the zebra finch, Bence Ölveczky (left) and his team of graduate student researchers Farhan Ali (center) and Timothy Otchy found that the brain uses two largely independent neural circuits to learn the temporal and spatial aspects of a motor skill. Credit: Jon Chase/Harvard Staff Photographer

Hitting the perfect tennis serve requires hours and hours of practice, but for scientists who study complex motor behaviors, there always has been a large unanswered question—what is the brain learning from those hours spent on the court? Is it simply the timing required to hit the perfect serve, or is it the precise path along which to move the hand?

The answer, Harvard researchers say, is both—but in separate circuits.

Bence Ölveczky, the John L. Loeb Associate Professor of the Natural Sciences, has found that the uses two largely independent neural circuits to learn the temporal and spatial aspects of a motor skill. The study is described in a Sept. 26 paper in Neuron.

"What we're studying is the structure of motor-skill ," Ölveczky said. "What we were able to show is that the brain divides something that's complex into modules—in this case for timing and for motor implementation—as a way to take advantage of the hierarchical structure of the motor system, and it imprints learning at the different levels independently."

To tease out how those independent circuits operate, Ölveczky and his colleagues turned to a creature well-known for its ability to learn—the zebra finch. The tiny birds are regularly used in studies of learning because each male learns to sing a unique song from its father.

In a series of experiments, Ölveczky's team used traditional conditioning techniques to change the timing of a bird's song by speeding up or slowing down certain "syllables" in the song. They could also change which vocal muscles were activated and have the bird sing at a higher or lower pitch.

"But when you change the pitch of a syllable, the duration doesn't change, and when you change the duration the pitch doesn't change," Ölveczky said. "It appears the for the two features are separate."

Additional evidence that the circuits for learning motor implementation and timing are distinct came when researchers lesioned the basal ganglia of the birds—the region of the brain long thought to play a critical role in song learning.

"The thinking had been that there was one circuit for song-learning in general," Ölveczky said. "We found that if we lesioned the basal ganglia and repeated the pitch-shift experiment, the bird could no longer use the information it got from our feedback to change its behavior—in other words, it couldn't learn."

Experiments aimed at changing the birds' timing, however, were just as effective, suggesting two separate learning circuits—with only one involving the basal ganglia.

Such independence and modularity is critical, Ölveczky said, because it allows different features of a behavior to be modified independently if circumstances change. Parallel learning of different features can also speed up the learning process and enable the flexibility we see in birdsong and many human motor skills.

"If you learn something—it could be your tennis serve, or it could be any behavior—and you need to slow it down or speed it up to fit some new contingency, you don't have to completely re-learn the whole thing, you can just change the timing, and everything else will remain exactly the same.

"In fact, 'slow practice,' a technique used by many piano and dance teachers, makes good use of this modularity," Ölveczky said. "Students are first taught to perform the movements of a piece slowly. Once they have learned it, all they need to do is get the timing right. The technique works because the two processes—motor implementation and timing—do not interfere with each other."

The hope among researchers, Ölveczky said, is that a better understanding of how birds learn complex motor tasks such as singing unique songs will help shed new light on the neural underpinnings of learning in humans.

"For us, this is inspiration to look at similar types of questions in mammals," he said. "The flexibility with which we can alter the spatial and temporal structure of our motor output is similar to songbirds, but our understanding of how the mammalian brain implements the underlying learning process is not anywhere near as advanced as for songbirds. The intriguing parallels in both circuitry and behavior, however, suggest a general principle of how the brain parses the motor skill ."

Explore further: Discovery helps to unlock brain's speech-learning mechanism

Related Stories

Discovery helps to unlock brain's speech-learning mechanism

September 4, 2013
USC scientists have discovered a population of neurons in the brains of juvenile songbirds that are necessary for allowing the birds to recognize the vocal sounds they are learning to imitate.

Songbirds' learning hub in brain offers insight into motor control

May 20, 2012
To learn its signature melody, the male songbird uses a trial-and-error process to mimic the song of its father, singing the tune over and over again, hundreds of times a day, making subtle changes in the pitch of the notes. ...

Songbirds shed light on brain circuits and learning

September 17, 2012
By studying how birds master songs used in courtship, scientists at Duke University have found that regions of the brain involved in planning and controlling complex vocal sequences may also be necessary for memorizing sounds ...

Brain study: Singing mice show signs of learning

October 10, 2012
Guys who imitate Luciano Pavarotti or Justin Bieber to get the girls aren't alone. Male mice may do a similar trick, matching the pitch of other males' ultrasonic serenades. The mice also have certain brain features, somewhat ...

Recommended for you

The neural codes for body movements

July 21, 2017
A small patch of neurons in the brain can encode the movements of many body parts, according to researchers in the laboratory of Caltech's Richard Andersen, James G. Boswell Professor of Neuroscience, Tianqiao and Chrissy ...

Faulty support cells disrupt communication in brains of people with schizophrenia

July 20, 2017
New research has identified the culprit behind the wiring problems in the brains of people with schizophrenia. When researchers transplanted human brain cells generated from individuals diagnosed with childhood-onset schizophrenia ...

Scientists discover combined sensory map for heat, humidity in fly brain

July 20, 2017
Northwestern University neuroscientists now can visualize how fruit flies sense and process humidity and temperature together through a "sensory map" within their brains, according to new research.

Scientists reveal how patterns of brain activity direct specific body movements

July 20, 2017
New research by Columbia scientists offers fresh insight into how the brain tells the body to move, from simple behaviors like walking, to trained movements that may take years to master. The discovery in mice advances knowledge ...

Team traces masculinization in mice to estrogen receptor in inhibitory neurons

July 20, 2017
Researchers at Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory (CSHL) have opened a black box in the brain whose contents explain one of the remarkable yet mysterious facts of life.

Speech language therapy delivered through the Internet leads to similar improvements as in-person treatment

July 20, 2017
Telerehabilitation helps healthcare professionals reach more patients in need, but some worry it doesn't offer the same quality of care as in-person treatment. This isn't the case, according to recent research by Baycrest.

2 comments

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

MrVibrating
1 / 5 (1) Sep 30, 2013
I've long supposed that delineation and parallel processing of spatial and temporal components is one of the chief benefits of having two hemispheres - itself a happy accident of developmental morphology - and that as such, this must be a distinguishing feature of lateralised faculties - ie. spatial components of words such as their spectral shapes on the left (Broca's area etc.) and temporal components on the right, such as prosody etc.

There seems to be a universal left-right bias for representing increasing magnitudes or values, too - across cultures. Plus we tend to under or overestimate quantities depending on whether our bodies are leaning left or right...

I think an absolute left/right segregation of spatial/temporal processing is probably unlikely, and may differ between faculties, but there's a definite general bias towards right-temporal and left-spatial.

While the benefits mentioned in the article are good examples, processing expediency is probably the driving factor..
MrVibrating
not rated yet Sep 30, 2013
Quite incidentally, one might further suppose that certain psychological conditions may be related to less-than-optimal delegation of temporal / spatial components between hemispheres - say, if both are processing components predominately in one or other domain only, their respective outputs may conflict...

I'd take that last suggestion with a large pinch of salt though, it's pure speculation..

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.