Researchers find predictor of child vocabulary

August 31, 2015, Northwestern University
Credit: Robert Kraft/public domain

At 12 months old, your infant's ability to group objects according to the names associated with them—as opposed to their appearance alone—offers a glimpse into how his or her vocabulary will develop by the time they are 18 months, Northwestern University researchers have found.

The research, by Brock Ferguson, Melanie Havy and leading developmental scientist Sandra Waxman, in the psychology department in the Weinberg College of Arts and Sciences, will appear in an upcoming issue of the open access journal Frontiers in Psychology.

"Our findings offer one piece of the very large puzzle that is development," said Ferguson, lead author of the study and a doctoral candidate in cognitive psychology.

"This is a big first step toward using these cognitive tasks to identify infants at risk for language delays."

The study examined whether individual differences in the precision of 12-month-olds' ability to link language and object categories was related to both their present and future vocabulary growth.

Ferguson offers a real-world analogy to the situation researchers create in the lab to measure infants' use of names as a guide to object categorization.

"Imagine you are taking your baby for a visit to the primate house at the zoo," he said. "There are gorillas, chimpanzees and monkeys in the primate house. Although all the primates look fairly similar, but we know them to be different because we refer to them by different names."

In that hypothetical scenario, 12-month-old babies who conclude that all of the primates belong in the same category, despite the fact that mom has referred to each by a different name, tend to have a less developed vocabulary, according to Ferguson's findings.

"In out study, babies who went ahead and grouped objects together even though the researchers had labeled them with distinct names were later found to know fewer words," Ferguson said.

Cognitive development is broadly defined as the process by which babies and young children learn about the world around them, and includes the acquisition of problem solving, memory, perception and, of course, language.

Working at the Project for Child Development, Waxman and her colleagues have already shown that talking to , even before they can speak, strengthens their language and .

"This new finding is the first to link infants' performance on our to their progress in learning new words," said Sandra Waxman, the Louis W. Menk Chair in Psychology, a professor of and a faculty fellow in the University's Institute for Policy Research.

Explore further: Infants using known verbs to learn new nouns

Related Stories

Infants using known verbs to learn new nouns

March 7, 2014
There is a lot that 19-month-old children can't do: They can't tie their shoes or get their mittens on the correct hands. But they can use words they do know to learn new ones.

Human speech's surprising influence on young infants

January 5, 2015
America's preoccupation with the "word gap"— the idea that parents in impoverished homes speak less to their children, which, in turn, predicts outcomes like school achievement and income later in life—has skyrocketed ...

Primate calls, like human speech, can help infants form categories

September 2, 2013
Human infants' responses to the vocalizations of non-human primates shed light on the developmental origin of a crucial link between human language and core cognitive capacities, a new study reports.

Nouns before verbs? New research agenda could help shed light on early language, cognitive development

March 25, 2013
Researchers are digging deeper into whether infants' ability to learn new words is shaped by the particular language being acquired.

Understanding how infants acquire new words across cultures

September 27, 2013
Infants show strong universals as they acquire their native language, but a recent study with infants acquiring Korean also reveals that there are striking language differences.

Recommended for you

Study shows how bias can influence people estimating the ages of other people

October 17, 2018
A trio of researchers from the University of New South Wales and Western Sydney University has discovered some of the factors involved when people make errors in estimating the ages of other people. In their paper published ...

Infants are more likely to learn when with a peer

October 16, 2018
Infants are more likely to learn from on-screen instruction when paired with another infant as opposed to viewing the lesson alone, according to a new study.

Researchers use brain cells in a dish to study genetic origins of schizophrenia

October 16, 2018
A study in Biological Psychiatry has established a new analytical method for investigating the complex genetic origins of mental illnesses using brain cells that are grown in a dish from human embryonic stem cells. Researchers ...

Income and wealth affect the mental health of Australians, study shows

October 16, 2018
Australians who have higher incomes and greater wealth are more likely to experience better mental health throughout their lives, new research led by the Bankwest Curtin Economics Centre has found.

Linguistic red flags from Facebook posts can predict future depression diagnoses

October 15, 2018
In any given year, depression affects more than 6 percent of the adult population in the United States—some 16 million people—but fewer than half receive the treatment they need. What if an algorithm could scan social ...

Early changes to synapse gene regulation may cause Alzheimer's disease

October 15, 2018
Alzheimer's disease (AD) is the most common form of dementia, involving memory loss and a reduction in cognitive abilities. Patients with AD develop multiple abnormal protein structures in their brains that are thought to ...

0 comments

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.