How a woman with amnesia defies conventional wisdom about memory

June 22, 2016, Johns Hopkins University
The hippocampus is a region of the brain largely responsible for memory formation. Credit: Salk Institute

She no longer recognizes a Van Gogh, but can tell you how to prepare a watercolor palette. She can't recall a single famous composer, but knows the purpose of a viola's bridge.

She hasn't flown a plane since 2007, when viral encephalitis destroyed her hippocampus, the part of the brain used to form new memories and retrieve old ones. And she couldn't describe a single trip she's ever taken. But in detail, she'll list the steps needed to keep a plane from stalling and where to find the rudder controls.

Johns Hopkins University cognitive scientists say the sharp contrasts in this patient's memory profile—her inability to remember facts about pursuits once vital to her life as an artist, musician and amateur aviator, while clearly remembering facts relevant to performing in these domains—suggest about how the brain stores knowledge is incorrect.

Conventional wisdom about memory firmly separates declarative knowledge, or memories about facts, from memories for skills, or "." For instance, a severe amnesiac with muscle memory might never forget how to ride a bike, but probably couldn't recall anything about the Tour de France. But because skilled performance, like playing music or flying airplanes, requires much more than mere muscle memory, and because this patient retained it despite losing most other aspects of her declarative memory, researchers conclude this type of skill-related declarative knowledge is different.

"There is such a contrast between her not being able to tell us anything about her former life and not being able to tell us anything about many aspects of art and music that she once knew well, but when we ask her to tell us how to do a watercolor, she is articulate and full of detail," said Barbara Landau, the Dick and Lydia Todd Professor of Cognitive Science at Johns Hopkins. "How can you talk about this knowledge of "how to" as distinct from declarative knowledge? It is declarative knowledge."

She no longer recognizes a Van Gogh, but can tell you how to prepare a watercolor palette. The sharp contrasts in an amnesia patient's memory profile suggest conventional wisdom about how the brain stores knowledge is incorrect. Credit: Len Turner/JHU

The findings, now online, are due to appear in an upcoming issue of the journal Cognitive Neuropsychology.

Before her illness, Lonni Sue Johnson, 64, was an accomplished artist whose portfolio included six New Yorker magazine covers. She was also an amateur violist who played in orchestras and chamber groups and a licensed single-engine airplane pilot who flew more than 400 flights and owned two planes. Her illness left her with severe brain damage and catastrophic memory impairment, including severe losses of memory about her previous life and severely restricted ability to learn new facts.

She has very little memory of her past—not even of her wedding day. She forgets having done something immediately after doing it. She also has very little memory for general world knowledge, including facts about the fields in which she once excelled.

To determine whether Johnson's "skill-related" memory was preserved despite extensive losses in memory for general world knowledge, the team tested her on her for facts related to performing four of her former top skills—art, music, flying and driving. They gave the same tests to people of similar age and experience in those areas, as well as to people with no experience in them.

The oral tests, of about 80 questions each, covered information about the techniques, equipment and terminology involved in performing the various skills. They included queries such as "How might one remove excess paint when painting with watercolor?" and "How should one touch the strings of an instrument to produce a harmonic?"

In art and driving, Johnson scored nearly as high as experts taking the test. In music and aviation, she did not perform as well, but knew considerably more than the novices.

"Although Johnson had not created watercolors, had not flown a plane, and had not driven since her illness, she could still describe how one would go about carrying out these activities," said Johns Hopkins cognitive scientist Michael McCloskey. "These findings suggest that skill-related knowledge can be spared even with dramatic losses in other kinds of knowledge."

Explore further: Forgetting is key to learning

More information: Emma Gregory et al, Declarative memory and skill-related knowledge: Evidence from a case study of amnesia and implications for theories of memory, Cognitive Neuropsychology (2016). DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2016.1172478

Related Stories

Forgetting is key to learning

December 18, 2015
Do you often feel overwhelmed with the amount of information coming at you? Forgotten your shopping list as soon as you've heard the sports results? Don't worry, it's all completely normal – and necessary – according ...

Disruption of sleep in children could hamper memory processes

April 15, 2015
Sleep disordered breathing can hamper memory processes in children, according to a new study.

Learning best when you rest: Sleeping after processing new info most effective, new study shows

March 23, 2012
Nodding off in class may not be such a bad idea after all. New research from the University of Notre Dame shows that going to sleep shortly after learning new material is most beneficial for recall.

What is your memory style? Tendency to remember episodic details versus facts is reflected in intrinsic brain patterns

December 10, 2015
Why is it that some people have richly detailed recollection of past experiences (episodic memory), while others tend to remember just the facts without details (semantic memory)?

Research finds hope for more accurate diagnosis of memory problems

July 30, 2014
More accurate tests could be created to diagnose diseases such as Alzheimer's or memory problems stemming from head injuries, leading to earlier intervention, according to new findings from the University of East Anglia (UEA).

The mechanisms of memory

June 21, 2016
Michy Kelly's fascination with how brains work began in high school when she noticed that she and her brother were polar opposites when it came to cognitive abilities.

Recommended for you

When storing memories, brain prioritizes those experiences that are most rewarding

November 20, 2018
The brain's ability to preserve memories lies at the heart of our basic human experience. But how does the brain's mechanism for memory make sure we remember the most significant events and not clog our minds with superfluous ...

Can genetic therapy help kids with Angelman syndrome overcome seizures?

November 20, 2018
Angelman syndrome is a genetic disease with no cure. Children grow up with severe intellectual disabilities and a range of other problems, arguably the worst of which are epileptic seizures. Now scientists at the UNC School ...

To predict the future, the brain has two clocks

November 20, 2018
That moment when you step on the gas pedal a split second before the light changes, or when you tap your toes even before the first piano note of Camila Cabello's "Havana" is struck. That's anticipatory timing.

Researchers hope to be able to replace dysfunctional brain cells

November 20, 2018
A new study by researchers at Karolinska Institutet supports the theory that replacement of dysfunctional immune cells in the brain has therapeutic potential for neurodegenerative diseases like ALS and Alzheimer's disease. ...

White matter pathway and individual variability in human stereoacuity

November 20, 2018
Researchers in the Center for Information and Neural Networks (CiNet), the National Institute of Information and Communications Technology and Osaka University have identified a human white matter pathway associated with ...

New immunotherapy improves MS symptoms

November 20, 2018
A world-first clinical trial of a new cellular immunotherapy for multiple sclerosis (MS) has improved symptoms and quality of life for the majority of patients.

2 comments

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

dan42day
not rated yet Jun 22, 2016
It appears that there may be a different mechanism for remembering skill memories. Accessing those memories might provide associative links to relevant factual memories that can no longer be accessed by other methods. Functional MRI testing might reveal the difference.
RobertKarlStonjek
not rated yet Jun 23, 2016
This is perfectly logical. If we assume that episodic memory is autobiographical in nature, that is, the set of all things that have a direct relationship to the self, then it is clear that this is what was lost. For objective phenomena, that is, phenomena that is true with or without a personal perspective or relationship, the memory is retained (most probably part of semantic memory).

Names of people can either be stored semantically, they are just things with no special meaning, or personally, as if you have a relationship with the name eg composers you like and respect.

This is a rather pleasing report as it is exactly what my own model of memory would predict. Having set it aside as unprovable (by me) I might dust it off and have another look.

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.