Detecting misinformation can improve memory later on

January 3, 2017, Association for Psychological Science
Credit: Association for Psychological Science

Exposure to false information about an event usually makes it more difficult for people to recall the original details, but new research suggests that there may be times when misinformation actually boosts memory. Research published in Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science, shows that people who actually notice that the misinformation is inconsistent with the original event have better memory for the event compared with people who never saw the misinformation in the first place.

"Our experiments show that misinformation can sometimes enhance memory rather than harm it," says psychological scientist Adam Putnam of Carleton College, lead author of the research. "These findings are important because they help explain why misinformation effects occur sometimes but not at other times - if people notice that the misinformation isn't accurate then they won't have a false memory."

In their first experiment, Putnam and colleagues had 72 undergraduate participants view six shows, each of which contained 50 photos portraying a particular event. After looking through the slide shows, the participants completed an unrelated "distractor" task for about five minutes and then read narrative descriptions for each slide in the previous slide shows.

For example, if the slide showed a thief finding $1 bills in a car, the description might be consistent (e.g., "He examined the bills, and saw they were all $1 bills"), neutral (e.g., "He examined the bills and saw they were all US currency"), or inconsistent (e.g., "He examined the bills and saw that they were all $20 bills") with the slide show.

After reading the descriptions and completing another distractor task, the participant then answered multiple-choice questions about what they remembered from the original slide shows, such as "What kind of bills were in the car?" The responses included a correct option ($1 bills), an incorrect option with misinformation from the narrative ($20 bills), or a different incorrect option ($5 bills). After making their selection, participants reported whether they had noticed any discrepancies between the original slide show and the narratives.

True to a general misinformation effect, people were most likely to choose the misinformation response when the detail in the narrative was inconsistent with the slide show.

But when participants reported remembering a change between the slide shows and the narrative, this deficit disappeared: Participants were more likely to select the correct response after seeing misinformation compared with seeing a neutral detail.

And when they reported that the narrative had contradicted the slide, participants were less likely to select the incorrect misinformation response for details that were inconsistent in the narrative compared with those that were neutral.

Although exposure to misinformation seemed to impair memory for the correct detail, detecting and remembering misinformation in the narrative seemed to improve participants' recognition later on.

A second experiment produced similar results, and additional analyses showed that how memorable a detail was seemed to make a difference. Details that were less memorable, relatively speaking, were more vulnerable to the misinformation effect.

These findings suggest that the relationship between misinformation and memory is more complex than we might have thought—mere exposure to misinformation doesn't automatically cue the effect:

"Classic interference theory in memory suggests that change is almost always bad for memory, but our study is one really clear example of how change can help memory in the right circumstances," Putnam explains.

"People may learn about false research and walk away thinking that false memories can easily be implanted about all sorts of events- that we're constantly remembering things that never happened," says Putnam. "Our research helps in showing that although false memories can occur with some regularity, it isn't a sure thing by any means."

Explore further: Half of people believe fake facts

More information: A. L. Putnam et al, When Misinformation Improves Memory: The Effects of Recollecting Change, Psychological Science (2016). DOI: 10.1177/0956797616672268

Related Stories

Half of people believe fake facts

December 7, 2016
Many people are prone to 'remembering' events that never happened, according to new research by the University of Warwick.

Misinformation: Report shows why it sticks and how to fix it

September 19, 2012
Childhood vaccines do not cause autism. Barack Obama was born in the United States. Global warming is confirmed by science. And yet, many people believe claims to the contrary.

Why we fall prey to misinformation

August 23, 2016
Even when we know better, we often rely on inaccurate or misleading information to make future decisions. But why are we so easily influenced by false statements such as "vaccinations cause autism" or "30 million illegal ...

Collaboration between media and medical journals often leads to misinformation and hysteria

December 8, 2016
When flawed clinical research is reported in the media with hype and sensationalism, it has the potential to have a devastating effect on patients, physicians, the scientific community and eventually society as a whole.

Recommended for you

Gender bias sways how we perceive competence in faces

December 7, 2018
Faces that are seen as competent are also perceived as more masculine, according to research published in Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

Internet therapy apps reduce depression symptoms, study finds

December 7, 2018
In a sweeping new study, Indiana University psychologists have found that a series of self-guided, internet-based therapy platforms effectively reduce depression.

Targeted cognitive training benefits patients with severe schizophrenia

December 7, 2018
Schizophrenia is among the most difficult mental illnesses to treat, in part because it is characterized by a wide range of dysfunction, from hallucinations and mood disorders to cognitive impairment, especially verbal and ...

Alterations in brain networks explain why some children are resilient to maltreatment

December 6, 2018
People who experience childhood maltreatment frequently have perturbations in their brain architecture, regardless of whether they develop psychiatric symptoms, but a study in Biological Psychiatry found additional alterations ...

PTSD study of combat veterans finds similar outcomes among common therapies

December 6, 2018
In a study among United States combat veterans, researchers found no significant difference between two of the most common treatments for post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and no benefit for combination treatment. The ...

Infections in the young may be tied to risk for mental illness: study

December 5, 2018
Could an infection make your child or teen prone to mental health issues?

0 comments

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.