Patients with OCD have difficulty learning when a stimulus is safe

March 6, 2017
Repetitive hand-washing is a common OCD symptom. Credit: Lars Klintwall Malmqvist/public domain

People who suffer from obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) are poorer at learning about the safety of a stimulus than healthy volunteers, which may contribute to their struggles to overcome compulsive behaviour, according to new research from the University of Cambridge.

OCD is a disorder characterised by intrusive thoughts and repetitive, irrational behaviours, for example an obsession with cleanliness leading to repetitive hand washing, or a fear that something terrible will happen if they don't check the door dozens of times, making leaving the house extremely difficult.

A common way of helping treat OCD is to expose people to something they consider threatening - for example, if their obsession is around cleanliness, they may be made to touch a toilet seat but then prevented from washing their hands. However, so-called 'exposure therapy' often only has limited success and compulsions can return in times of stress. This new research, published today in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, may explain why memories about safety don't stick.

In this study, researchers at Cambridge's Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute tested 43 OCD patients and 35 matched to see how well those people with OCD were able to reverse their thinking when a previously threatening stimulus became safe and vice versa, to examine safety versus threat learning as well as cognitive flexibility, which is thought to be significantly compromised in patients with OCD.

Volunteers lay in a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scanner, which measures , while successively being shown one of two faces: when shown the red face, nothing happened, but when shown the green face, the volunteer would sometimes receive a mild . By measuring changes in skin conductance caused by tiny amounts of sweat, the researchers were able to see whether the volunteers learned which stimulus was safe and which threatening.

After a period of time, the researchers swapped the stimuli - now, the red face was paired with an electric shock while the green face was safe.

The researchers found that while OCD patients were able to learn initially which stimulus was threatening, they never learned that the second stimulus was safe - in fact, they seemed to pay little attention to this safe stimulus. When the stimuli were reversed, participants were unable to differentiate between the previously threatening stimulus and the newly threatening stimulus. This was also reflected in their brain activity - OCD patients showed a lack of activity in an area at the front of the brain known as the when viewing the safe stimulus.

"Our study suggests that something is going wrong in the brains of people with OCD when they are learning what is safe, and this in turn affects how they perceive threats under updated circumstances," explains Dr Annemieke Apergis-Schoute, the study's first author. "This needs to be taken into consideration when we're developing future therapies to tackle the disorder. Current exposure therapies may help the patient take control over their compulsions, but our work suggests that they might never learn that their compulsions are unnecessary and they may return in times of stress."

In a second study, published recently in Biological Psychiatry, Cambridge researchers showed that this cognitive inflexibility might be in part a result of a lack of 'chatter' between specific brain areas.

The research, led by PhD student Matilde Vaghi, found that poor connectivity within some of the brain's key networks as measured in an fMRI scanner while the patient was at rest may account for this inflexibility. It also may account for OCD patients' poor goal-directed abilities (where we consciously act with a goal in mind - for example, when driving home and our route is disrupted, forcing us to take an unfamiliar route). Both are related to common symptoms of OCD.

The researchers found disrupted connectivity within discrete frontostriatal circuits - neural pathways that connect the front of the brain with the basal ganglia (responsible for important functions such as the control of movement and 'executive functions' such as decision-making, learning and habit formation). They believe these may underlie the repetitive behaviours seen in OCD.

Professor Trevor Robbins, Head of Psychology at Cambridge, senior author on both studies, says: "When we look at this two studies together, we can see that there is a clear imbalance between key regions at the front of the brain in people with OCD. These may underlie some of the symptoms of inflexibility that we commonly see in patients with this condition."

Explore further: OCD patients' brains light up to reveal how compulsive habits develop

More information: Apergis-Schoute, AM et al.Neural basis of impaired safety signaling in Obsessive Compulsive Disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences; 6 Mar 2017; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1609194114

Vaghi, MM et al. Specific Frontostriatal Circuits for Impaired Cognitive Flexibility and Goal-Directed Planning in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Evidence From Resting-State Functional Connectivity. Biological Psychiatry; Aug 2016; DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.08.009

Related Stories

OCD patients' brains light up to reveal how compulsive habits develop

December 19, 2014
Misfiring of the brain's control system might underpin compulsions in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), according to researchers at the University of Cambridge, writing in the American Journal of Psychiatry.

New research provides insight into how obsessive-compulsive disorder develops

May 23, 2011
(Medical Xpress) -- New scientific evidence challenges a popular conception that behaviours such as repetitive hand-washing, characteristic of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), are carried out in response to disturbing ...

Less is more: Exposure to stimuli for overcoming phobia

February 6, 2017
A team of investigators, led by Bradley S. Peterson, MD, director of the Institute for the Developing Mind at Children's Hospital Los Angeles, and Paul Siegel, PhD, associate professor of psychology at Purchase College of ...

Fast fMRI tracks brain activity during human thought for first time

December 2, 2016
By significantly increasing the speed of functional MRI (fMRI), researchers funded by the National Institute of Biomedical Imaging and Bioengineering (NIBIB) have been able to image rapidly fluctuating brain activity during ...

Fear, safety and the role of sleep in human PTSD

August 27, 2014
The effectiveness of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) treatment may hinge significantly upon sleep quality, report researchers at the University of California, San Diego School of Medicine and Veterans Affairs San Diego ...

Recommended for you

Nature or nurture? Innate social behaviors in the mouse brain

October 18, 2017
Adult male mice have a simple repertoire of innate, or instinctive, social behaviors: When encountering a female, a male mouse will try to mate with it, and when encountering another male, the mouse will attack. The animals ...

Brain activity predicts crowdfunding outcomes better than self-reports

October 18, 2017
Surveys and self-reports are a time-honored way of trying to predict consumer behavior, but they have limitations. People often give socially desirable answers or they simply don't know or remember things clearly.

Brain-machine interfaces to treat neurological disease

October 18, 2017
Since the 19th century at least, humans have wondered what could be accomplished by linking our brains – smart and flexible but prone to disease and disarray – directly to technology in all its cold, hard precision. Writers ...

Changing stroke definitions is causing chaos, warns professor

October 18, 2017
Proposals to change the definitions of stroke and related conditions are causing confusion and chaos in clinical practice and research, a Monash University associate professor has warned.

Navigational view of the brain thanks to powerful X-rays

October 18, 2017
If brain imaging could be compared to Google Earth, neuroscientists would already have a pretty good "satellite view" of the brain, and a great "street view" of neuron details. But navigating how the brain computes is arguably ...

'Wasabi receptor' for pain discovered in flatworms

October 18, 2017
A Northwestern University research team has discovered how scalding heat and tissue injury activate an ancient "pain" receptor in simple animals. The findings could lead to new strategies for analgesic drug design for the ...

1 comment

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

RobertKarlStonjek
not rated yet Mar 07, 2017
Can signing executive orders become obsessive and compulsive?

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.