This article has been reviewed according to Science X's editorial process and policies. Editors have highlighted the following attributes while ensuring the content's credibility:

fact-checked

trusted source

proofread

Research aims to improve understanding of children's procrastination

Research aims to improve understanding of children's procrastination
According to the multicomponent definition of procrastination, procrastination occurs when all the following criteria are met: the start or completion of a task is delayed; there is an intention to complete that task; the delay decision is voluntary; the delay is unnecessary; and there is an expectation of future negative consequences (case 1). According to this restrictive view of procrastination, delaying an intended task because of unexpected challenges (case 2) or impulsive behavior (case 3) do not constitute procrastination because they do not meet all multicomponent definition criteria. Credit: Nature Reviews Psychology (2024). DOI: 10.1038/s44159-024-00341-w

It's not uncommon to catch preschoolers dilly-dallying when they're supposed to be tidying or school-aged children putting off homework for more desirable tasks. But are these forms of procrastination? And do they influence future adult tendencies? These are questions Brock Professor of Psychology Caitlin Mahy is taking steps to answer.

"Research on in is an emerging field," she says. "The same measures we use to study adult procrastination don't necessarily apply to children at different stages of development."

Mahy has co-published a paper calling for procrastination definitions, theory and interventions to be informed by studies on adults and children, which have so far run parallel to one another.

By taking this approach, researchers will more deeply understand how procrastination plays out in young children as they mature, leading to better early-life interventions, she says.

The paper maps out children's typical procrastination-type behaviors in various stages, beginning with preschoolers (ages two to four), who put off tasks and household routines; older preschoolers (ages five and six), who delay doing homework and ; elementary and middle-school children (ages seven to 13), who experience an increase in academic procrastination; and high school teens (ages 14 to 18), for whom procrastination is prominent at school as competing demands on time increase.

As time passes, children generate more simultaneous goals, while parents play a decreasing role in generating, directing and regulating goals for their children.

Researchers probing for a greater understanding of these behaviors mostly rely on reports from parents describing what they see with their children compared to studies on adult procrastinators who fill out questionnaires on their own behaviors, says Mahy.

The standard definition of procrastination includes five criteria: there is an intention to perform the ; the task is delayed; the delay is voluntary; the delay is unnecessary; and future negative outcomes are expected.

"Very young children have difficulty reporting on their intentions," says Mahy. "Even if we study their behavior, you don't necessarily know the child's intentions, you can't read their mind to see if they've anticipated future consequences, you're just observing their delay in performing tasks."

Because young children's abilities to be introspective and fully understand the consequences of their delays are absent, their behaviors can't be strictly classified as being "procrastination, according to the current, prevalent definition," says Mahy.

"Instead, we can say these may be early pre-cursors or represent emerging procrastination behavior."

In addition to reshaping the definition of procrastination to capture children's experience, Mahy and her team describe how theory and intervention could be further improved by considering adult and child research.

They outline several , such as emotion regulation, that likely contribute to the development of procrastination. They also recommend more research on the development of children's procrastination over time and across age ranges, as well as the creation of more objective ways of assessing children's procrastination.

These and other findings are included in "Mutual implications of procrastination research in adults and children for theory and intervention"—published in the September issue of Nature Reviews Psychology—in which co-authors Yuko Munakata from the University of California Davis and Akira Miyake from the University of Colorado Boulder review a broad range of research on adults' and children's procrastination.

For parents wishing to help their children avoid procrastinating, Mahy offers these tips:

  • Use a "starting ritual" such as a countdown or the "Pomodoro technique," which involves five-minute breaks after 25 minutes of doing the task.
  • Clear distractions, such as toys or screens, in the immediate environment.
  • Break down longer-term deadlines into shorter tasks and chunks of time.
  • Make tasks more fun to do and accomplish.

More information: Caitlin E. V. Mahy et al, Mutual implications of procrastination research in adults and children for theory and intervention, Nature Reviews Psychology (2024). DOI: 10.1038/s44159-024-00341-w

Provided by Brock University
Citation: Research aims to improve understanding of children's procrastination (2024, September 11) retrieved 11 September 2024 from https://medicalxpress.com/news/2024-09-aims-children-procrastination.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further

Researchers find early emergence of procrastination in children

0 shares

Feedback to editors