Where 'where it's at' is at in the brain: Study in rats identifies region that associates objects and space

Door open left rear; table left; glass fish on first shelf: In rats, the postrhinal cortex keeps track of places and objects and sends other parts of the brain vital positioning information. "The POR," Rebecca Burwell says, "is not just a relay station for spatial information." Credit: David Orenstein/Brown University

Conventional wisdom in brain research says that you just used your hippocampus to answer that question, but that might not be the whole story. The context of place depends on not just how you got there, but also the things you see around you. A new study in Neuron provides evidence that a different part of the brain is important for understanding where you are based on the spatial layout of the objects in that place. The finding, in rats, has a direct analogy to primate neuroanatomy.

"Understanding how and where context is represented in the is important," said study senior author Rebecca Burwell, professor of psychology and neuroscience at Brown University. "Context, or the place in which events occur, is the hallmark of , but context is more than a place or a location. This room, for example, has a window, furniture, and other objects. You walk into a room and all that information helps you remember what happened there."

Pinpointing where the brain puts together objects and places to form a context could also matter for treating traumatic brain injuries or , such as schizophrenia and depression, that involve that part of the brain, said Burwell, who is also affiliated with the Brown Institute for .

"We know that contextual representations are disrupted in , particularly schizophrenia and depression," Burwell said. "Individuals with these disorders have trouble using context to plan actions or choose appropriate behaviors."

Part of the funding for the study came from a defense department grant aimed at laying the basic neuroscience framework for ultimately repairing traumatic brain damage.

Ratting out dogma

The particular region of Burwell's interest is called the postrhinal , or POR, in rats. It is directly analogous to the parahippocampal cortex, or PHC, in primates including humans. Neuroscientists already credit the POR, and the PHC, with a significant role in encoding spatial context, mainly by providing spatial information to the hippocampus ("There is a wall five feet to the left"), but they have presumed that another brain region, the perirhinal cortex (PER), provides the hippocampus with information about objects ("I see a fish sculpture").

"The dogma is that this spatial and nonspatial information is segregated," Burwell said. That is, until those two streams of information are finally integrated by the hippocampus ("The fish sculpture is on the left-hand wall shelf.").

But previous research has shown that the PER and POR talk directly to each other. Also, damage to either of those brain regions results in rats that become confused about context. For those, and other reasons, Burwell's team hypothesized that the POR links objects to places to encode spatial context upstream of the .

"That's what we think is happening in the POR," she said. "It's integrating information about place and information about objects to characterize the spatial layout of a local context."

That object in that place

To find out, the authors surgically inserted electrodes near dozens of POR neurons in each of five rats. The rats were trained to perform a series of experimental tasks in a simple bowtie-shaped "maze." Each time, they had to choose between a pair of images (objects) projected into two corners (places) of the maze floor. When the rats moved to the "correct" object, they were rewarded with sips of 2% fat chocolate milk.

The researchers found that neurons in the POR responded in patterns of electrical activity, or "spiking," not only when the rats looked at objects or scurried to places, but also when specific objects (like one associated with a reward) turned up in specific places. That is, neurons often spiked when a particular object appeared in a particular corner.

The rats even exhibited a specific neural response for "egocentric" movements. In other words, the rats' POR neurons didn't just account for object location in an objective "this side of the maze or that side" sense, but also for a self-specific "on my left or on my right" basis, indicating that the POR considers space relative to the self, regardless of where a particular object might be in the room.

Networks of POR also tended to fire rhythmically at a specific frequency, called "theta," that neuroscientists think coordinates long-distance communication across the brain.

"It was a surprise to see this whopping theta in the POR," Burwell said.

Theta rhythm was particularly strong when chose an unrewarded object. Burwell said that indicates the POR may be responsible for telling other parts of the brain when the wrong choice of object and place has been made.

"The POR," Burwell said, "is not just a relay station for spatial information."

It appears to be much more.

Related Stories

Study: Theta rhythm reduces seizure rate

Jun 20, 2006

Texas scientists say the brain's septum helps stop epileptic seizures by inducing electrical activity in another area of the brain called the hippocampus.

Do mammals think in 3-D?

Jul 14, 2008

A team of neuroscientists at University College London (UCL) has begun to discover how the brain maps three-dimensional space. The work could one day aid in the understanding and treatment of Alzheimer’s disease, which ...

Recommended for you

New test to help brain injury victims recover

20 hours ago

A dynamic new assessment for helping victims of trauma to the brain, including those suffering from progressive conditions such as dementia, has been developed by a clinical neuropsychologist at the University ...

See-through sensors open new window into the brain

22 hours ago

(Medical Xpress)—Developing invisible implantable medical sensor arrays, a team of University of Wisconsin-Madison engineers has overcome a major technological hurdle in researchers' efforts to understand ...

User comments

Adjust slider to filter visible comments by rank

Display comments: newest first

Tausch
not rated yet Dec 05, 2012
"You walk into a room and all that information helps you remember what happened there. - Rebecca Burwell"


...and 'obscures' the information that led you into the room in the first place. The trivial labels/words are "forgot/forgetting".

Now enter your new office - a windowless, featureless room where the only (randomly placed) object in the room is the object you had in mind before entering the room.

Did you 'forget' what you were after?
Or what you wanted to do there?
The moment you entered?
(Provoke readers here. Be difficult. Say yes.)

Anyway, what does this tell you?
I'm sure you will remember.

http://medicalxpr...ays.html
Walking through doorways causes forgetting, new research shows.
Now refuted.

MrVibrating
not rated yet Dec 05, 2012
...indicating that the POR considers space relative to the self, regardless of where a particular object might be in the room.


A couple of suggestions the hypothesis might predict:

Enlarged POR in felines, since their mapping and navigational systems are egocentric - cats remember where their food bowl is relative to their current location, rather than its position in the kitchen, for example.

Similarly, speakers of Guugu Yimithirr would be expected to have a reduced POR, since their navigatory systems are purely cardinal - they have no concept of "left" or "right", "in front of" or "behind" - instead relying exclusively on north, south, east and west. If a tree is in front of me, and then i rotate 180°, it's now behind me. But a native Guugu Yimithirr speaker would still describe the tree as being to the east, say, regardless of their own orientation relative to it.
Tausch
not rated yet Dec 05, 2012
http://medicalxpr...tal.html

Where lateralization of function proponents lost ground.
Where volume of function is not even wrong - see above comment.