Pitch imperfect? How the brain decodes pitch may improve cochlear implants

November 22, 2017 by Kim Eckart, University of Washington
A study examining how the brain decodes pitch could inform further development of cochlear implants. Credit: University of Washington

Picture yourself with a friend in a crowded restaurant. The din of other diners, the clattering of dishes, the muffled notes of background music, the voice of your friend, not to mention your own – all compete for your brain's attention.

For many people, the brain can automatically distinguish the noises, identifying the sources and recognizing what they "say" and mean thanks to, among other features of , pitch.

But for someone who wears a cochlear implant, a surgically implanted electronic device that restores a sense of hearing, pitch is only weakly conveyed. For decades, scientists have debated how, exactly, humans perceive pitch, and how the ear and the brain transmit pitch information in a sound. There are two prevalent theories: place and time. The "time " theory argues that pitch is a matter of auditory nerve fiber firing rate, while the "place code" theory focuses on where in the inner ear a sound activates.

Now a new study bolsters support for the place code. These findings, published in the September issue of the Journal of Neuroscience, could inform further development of the cochlear implant. The paper's lead author is Bonnie Lau, a speech-language pathologist and postdoctoral fellow at the University of Washington Institute for Learning & Brain Sciences. Here, her research focuses on development of the human auditory system.

Pitch is one of the basic aspects of sound. It roughly corresponds to the periodicity of sound waves; sounds with a higher pitch have a higher repetition rate. Most often associated with music and voices, pitch contributes an aesthetic quality to what we hear. Think of an ocean, or a symphony; without pitch, Lau explained, "the things we love to listen to, those aesthetic aspects will be changed." Pitch also functions as a cue to distinguish sounds, especially in noisy places. It makes listening in real-world environments like restaurants and public transportation more difficult when we don't have access to pitch," she added.

Pinning pitch perception on a "place code" provides opportunities for improvement of that would not be possible if pitch were perceived only through a "time code."

Here's how the two codes work:

  • In a time code, which relies on a phenomenon called "phase locking," auditory nerve fibers respond to a time-based pattern in a sound wave by firing at the same place every cycle, transmitting information to the brain—a process that works only up to a certain frequency. But beyond a certain repetition rate, the auditory nerve fibers can't follow the periodicity in a sound.
  • In a place code, different frequencies activate different parts of the inner ear, with pitch organized from high to low, like a musical scale. Where the activation is indicates the pitch of a sound.

For Lau's experiment, researchers tested 19 people (average age: 22) with a range of musical training (from no formal training to 15 years' worth). Musical experience, Lau said, turned out to have no clear correlation with pitch perception in this study.

The participants listened to and compared a series of high-frequency tones (greater than 8,000 Hz) with specialized headphones in a soundproof booth; after each tone, participants used a computer to indicate which sound was higher in pitch. Researchers chose only very high-frequency tones, embedded in background noise, in order to eliminate the possibility of a time code and focused instead on whether a place code was at work. And when these ultra-high frequency pure tones were combined in a harmonic complex (think musical notes), participants' improved significantly.

"Our findings show that even when timing information is not available, you can still hear pitch," Lau said.

The design of new technologies, then, could benefit from this finding, she said. Cochlear implants currently convey little pitch information to the user, but these results suggest that enhancing place information alone has the potential to improve perception from a cochlear implant. A separate study, supported by the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders, is exploring whether a shortened electrode array in the implant can target exclusively high-frequency sounds.

Explore further: Our ability to focus on one voice in crowds is triggered by voice pitch

More information: Bonnie K. Lau et al. Superoptimal Perceptual Integration Suggests a Place-Based Representation of Pitch at High Frequencies, The Journal of Neuroscience (2017). DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1507-17.2017

Related Stories

Our ability to focus on one voice in crowds is triggered by voice pitch

October 10, 2017
Scientists have discovered that a group of neurons in the brain's auditory stem help us to tune into specific conversations in a crowded room.

Humans probably not alone in how we perceive melodic pitch

December 28, 2015
The specialized human ability to perceive the sound quality known as 'pitch' can no longer be listed as unique to humans. Researchers at Johns Hopkins report new behavioral evidence that marmosets, ancient monkeys, appear ...

New strategy lets cochlear implant users hear music

October 9, 2013
For many, music is a universal language that unites people when words cannot. But for those who use cochlear implants—technology that allows deaf and hard of hearing people to comprehend speech—hearing music remains extremely ...

Cochlear implant users can hear, feel the beat in music

January 26, 2015
People who use cochlear implants for profound hearing loss do respond to certain aspects of music, contrary to common beliefs and limited scientific research, says a research team headed by an investigator at Georgetown University ...

Recommended for you

Researchers discover novel mode of neurotransmitter-based communication

May 21, 2018
Researchers at the University of California, Irvine School of Medicine have discovered the first example of a novel mode of neurotransmitter-based communication. The discovery, published in Nature Communications, challenges ...

Reading the minds of pilots on the fly

May 21, 2018
After a plane engine blew apart at 32,000 feet in the air last month, the pilot flying Southwest Flight 1380 safely brought the Boeing 737 to an emergency landing in Philadelphia. Captain Tammie Jo Shults was heralded a hero, ...

Mice regrow brain tissue after stroke with bioengineered gel

May 21, 2018
In a first-of-its-kind finding, a new stroke-healing gel helped regrow neurons and blood vessels in mice with stroke-damaged brains, UCLA researchers report in the May 21 issue of Nature Materials.

Vascular risk interacts with amyloid levels to increase age-related cognitive decline

May 21, 2018
Risk factors for heart disease and stroke appear to hasten the risk of cognitive decline in normal older individuals with evidence of very early Alzheimer's-disease-associated changes in the brain. Vascular risk factors increase ...

Casting light on better bowels

May 21, 2018
A novel light therapy is being investigated as an alternative to prescription drugs to relieve chronic constipation and other intestinal disorders which affect millions of patients around the world.

How social isolation transforms the brain

May 17, 2018
Chronic social isolation has debilitating effects on mental health in mammals—for example, it is often associated with depression and post-traumatic stress disorder in humans. Now, a team of Caltech researchers has discovered ...

0 comments

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.