How much will we pay for something? Depends on the value of what we've just encountered

November 13, 2017, New York University
Credit: CC0 Public Domain

The value of the products we encounter influences how much we'll subsequently pay for other items, new neuroscience research has found. The results point to a previously undetected factor that affects consumer behavior.

The study, which appears in the journal Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS), shows that when we come across low-valued items, we're willing to pay more for products we later face; by contrast, when we see high-valued items, we'll pay less for products we view in the future.

"How people value an item is not a simple function of that item alone," explains Kenway Louie, a research assistant professor at New York University's Center for Neural Science and one of the authors of the paper. "The valuation process is inherently relative, with people valuing the same exact item more or less depending on the environment they recently inhabited. Our study shows that rewards cannot be evaluated in isolation, but instead must be viewed through the lens of the recent past."

It's been long established that our brains process information by relying on comparisons rather than on absolute judgments. This dynamic is fundamental in , where our perception of in the world depends on the context in which those stimuli appear. For example, a gray square will appear darker to someone coming in from bright sunlight than to someone who's been in a dark room.

Less clear is how sensory processing can influence decision-making—or, specifically, evaluations we make.

In the PNAS work, the researchers, who also included NYU's Paul Glimcher, a professor of neuroscience, and Mel Khaw, an NYU doctoral student at the time of the study and now a post-doctoral researcher at Columbia University, studied how different environments could affect how people valued food items.

To do so, a set of experimental viewed 30 different food items on a computer screen and reported how much they would pay for those items. The researchers then calculated these responses to establish a ranking of all the items—from lowest to highest price, based on the subjects' answers.

Following this, the study's subjects underwent a series of trials in which they viewed only the 10 lowest-valued items—a "low-value" condition labeled "the adapt block."

The researchers then repeated the first part of the experiment, once again asking the subjects how much they would pay for each of the 30 items. Here, the study sought to determine if viewing the lowest-valued items would cause the subjects to say they'd pay more for these 30 items than they originally indicated. As predicted, after viewing the lower-priced items, the subjects did indeed say they'd pay more for these 30 items than first stated.

Next, the researchers repeated the adapt block—but, this time, subjects were shown the 10 highest-value items (a high-value, or rich, environment). Conversely, and as hypothesized, after seeing the higher-priced items, the subjects said they'd pay less for all 30 items than previously indicated.

"Collectively, these findings provide the first evidence that adaptation extends to the economic value we place on products," explains Louie. "Moreover, they suggest that adaptation is a universal feature of cognitive information processing."

Explore further: Neuroscientists discover way to increase product value without making changes to it

More information: Mel W. Khaw el al., "Normalized value coding explains dynamic adaptation in the human valuation process," PNAS (2017). www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1715293114

Related Stories

Neuroscientists discover way to increase product value without making changes to it

March 10, 2014
(Medical Xpress)—A team of researchers working at the University of Texas has discovered a way to cause the perceived value of a product to rise, without changing the product itself: add a button that makes noise. In their ...

Healthy concession stand makeovers are a game changer

January 18, 2017
Concession stands at school sporting events are often overlooked by those advocating for healthy school food. However, a new study highlights how concession stands can benefit from adding on healthy items.

Limited short-term memory caused by 'interference' from similar items seen earlier, says study

June 7, 2017
Our short-term memory is severely limited in everyday experience, but according to a new study from City, University of London and the Hungarian Academy of Science, it has no intrinsic limits when it comes to remembering ...

Ghrelin increases willingness to pay for food

July 12, 2011
Research to be presented at the upcoming annual meeting of the Society for the Study of Ingestive Behavior (SSIB), the foremost society for research into all aspects of eating and drinking behavior, suggests that ghrelin, ...

Recommended for you

Dehydration alters human brain shape and activity, slackens task performance

August 21, 2018
When dehydration strikes, part of the brain can swell, neural signaling can intensify, and doing monotonous tasks can get harder.

Researchers discover novel subtype of multiple sclerosis

August 21, 2018
Cleveland Clinic researchers have discovered a new subtype of multiple sclerosis (MS), providing a better understanding of the individualized nature of the disease.

'It's all in the eyes': The role of the amygdala in the experience and perception of fear

August 21, 2018
Researchers have long believed that the amygdala, an almond-shaped structure in the brain, is central to the experience and perception of fear. Studies initiated in the 1990s of a patient with a rare condition affecting the ...

Study sheds light on how brain lets animals hunt for food by following smells

August 21, 2018
Most animals have a keen sense of smell, which assists them in everyday tasks. Now, a new study led by researchers at NYU School of Medicine sheds light on exactly how animals follow smells.

Powerful molecules provide new findings about Huntington's disease

August 21, 2018
Researchers at Lund University in Sweden have discovered a direct link between the protein aggregation in nerve cells that is typical for neurodegenerative diseases, and the regulation of gene expression in Huntington's disease. ...

Largest brain study of 62,454 scans identifies drivers of brain aging

August 21, 2018
In the largest known brain imaging study, scientists from Amen Clinics (Costa Mesa, CA), Google, John's Hopkins University, University of California, Los Angeles and the University of California, San Francisco evaluated 62,454 ...

0 comments

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.