Separate brain systems cooperate during learning, study finds

February 21, 2018 by Kevin Stacey, Brown University
New research uses EEG and a specialized experimental setup to show how working memory and reinforcement learning work together as people learn to perform new tasks. Credit: Frank Lab / Brown University

A new study by Brown University researchers shows that two different brain systems work cooperatively as people learn.

The study, published in Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, focused on the interplay of two very different modes of learning a new task: reinforcement learning and working . Reinforcement learning is an "under-the-hood" process in which people gradually learn which actions to take by processing rewards and punishments at the neural level, and then choosing the one that works best on average—even if the person is not aware of it. In contrast, working memory involves keeping previous actions and their outcomes in mind to more rapidly and flexibly improve performance.

"People have largely interpreted these systems as working independently or as competing with each other in the learning process," said Michael Frank, a professor in Brown's Department of Cognitive, Linguistic and Psychological Sciences and co-author of the paper. "But we show that the two work together, with neural signals underlying working memory helping to guide those that support reinforcement learning."

Anne Collins, an assistant professor at the University of California, Berkeley, led the work when she was a postdoctoral researcher working with Frank, who directs the Initiative for Computation in Brain and Mind in the Brown Institute for Brain Science. Collins and Frank developed an experimental method designed to isolate the brain signals associated with each of the two systems.

For the study, 40 study participants were shown a series of symbols on a screen and asked, for each symbol, to press a particular button on a keyboard. They weren't told which key was the right one for each symbol. They had to learn it. When they got it right, they were rewarded with points. Over repeated trials, the participants came to learn which keys corresponded with which symbols.

In order to distinguish the contributions from reinforcement learning and working memory, the researchers set up problems with different numbers of symbols, ranging from two to six, and participants had to learn which button to press for each of them. Generally, people can only hold three or four items in working memory at a time, and only for short periods of time. So when the number of symbols or the delay increases, the contribution of working memory to the learning process should diminish.

As the participants performed the tasks, an EEG cap recorded signals from the brain, and the authors applied statistical methods to extract those signals related to one learning system or the other.

The study showed that when memory demands were high, the signals in the brain correlated to reinforcement learning actually got stronger. In other words, when the working memory system was overtaxed, the reinforcement learning system became more important in the learning process. In contrast, when participants could hold information in mind, signals associated with reinforcement learning were weaker, suggesting an increased role for working memory.

The researchers also found that they could decode from the brain signals in a particular trial whether information was likely to be in memory or not. That too traded off with the neural marker of reinforcement learning.

Those findings, the researchers say, suggest that the two systems aren't working independently.

"If they were completely independent of each other, we'd expect the signals associated with reinforcement learning to stay the same regardless of memory demands," Frank said. "But that's not we see, and that's a sign that the two systems are interacting."

But on its own, that finding didn't reveal the nature of that interaction—whether it's cooperative or competitive. Was working memory shoving the reinforcement learning into the background in trials when the information could be readily accessible in mind? Or could it be that working memory helps to augment reinforcement learning? To figure that out, the researchers looked how the brain signals associated with reinforcement learning changed as the learning process unfolded from trial to trial.

The reinforcement learning system is driven by what's known as "reward prediction error" or RPE, and it's the signal the researchers used to track the reinforcement learning process. RPE represents the extent to which the reward that results from an action exceeds one's expectations. Take for example a study participant trying to figure out which button to press when they see a given symbol. If they happen to guess right and get rewarded with points, that outcome is surprisingly good and produces a high RPE.

In the , the reinforcement learning system uses the neurotransmitter dopamine to encode RPE. A high RPE—meaning a surprisingly good outcome—is associated with a large release of dopamine. The reinforcement learning system uses that dopamine flood as a signal to update our understanding of what actions we should take to get a given reward. When we repeat that action subsequently, we're less surprised by the reward and so the RPE is lower. As RPE continues to diminish, the system eventually stops updating, and in so doing, settles upon an appropriate action.

One scenario for how working memory could be interacting with reinforcement learning is by attenuating reward expectations, making them more quickly come into line with actual rewards. In that way, working memory could be working cooperatively to speed the reinforcement learning .

The study found strong evidence for just that scenario. During repeated trials at small set sizes where working memory is active, associated with RPE started out high in the first few trials, and then quickly dropped off—a sign that cognitive processes are informing the neural signaling associated with reinforcement learning. In contrast, if working memory were merely suppressing , one wouldn't expect to see the quick drop in RPE.

The results, Frank said, provide some of the first concrete evidence for cooperation between these two systems.

"Thinking of these not as separate systems but as one big integrated system changes our understanding of the basic science of how people and animals learn," Frank said. "It might help us make better predictions about how the overall is affected in people who have deficits in either of these systems."

And that, Frank said, could one day lead to better treatments for learning impairments.

Explore further: Working memory hinders learning in schizophrenia

More information: Anne G. E. Collins et al. Within- and across-trial dynamics of human EEG reveal cooperative interplay between reinforcement learning and working memory, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (2018). DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1720963115

Related Stories

Working memory hinders learning in schizophrenia

October 7, 2014
A new study pinpoints working memory as a source of learning difficulties in people with schizophrenia.

Artificial agent designs quantum experiments

January 19, 2018
On the way to an intelligent laboratory, physicists from Innsbruck and Vienna present an artificial agent that autonomously designs quantum experiments. In initial experiments, the system has independently (re)discovered ...

Teaching machines to direct traffic through deep reinforcement learning

August 17, 2016
Rush hour—the dreaded time of day when traffic conditions seem bent on making you late. As your car slowly creeps in line behind countless others stuck at a stop light, you think to yourself, "Why aren't these lights changing ...

Brain study reveals how teens learn differently than adults

October 5, 2016
Scientists have uncovered a unique feature of the adolescent brain that enriches teens' ability to learn and form memories: the coordinated activity of two distinct brain regions. This observation, which stands in contrast ...

Research unlocks clues to language-based learning in children

December 12, 2016
According to the National Center for Learning Disabilities (NCLD), one in five individuals are impacted by language-based learning disabilities—one of the most common being dyslexia, which involves difficulty in reading ...

DeepMind researchers boost AI learning speed with UNREAL agent

November 21, 2016
(Tech Xplore)—At Google's DeepMind, a team has made AI inroads in speed and performance.

Recommended for you

What prevents remyelination? New stem cell research reveals a critical culprit

December 18, 2018
New research on remyelination, the spontaneous regeneration of the brain's fatty insulator that keeps neurons communicating, could lead to a novel approach to developing treatments for multiple sclerosis (MS) and other inflammatory ...

Genetic changes tied to rare brain bleeds in babies

December 18, 2018
(HealthDay)—Researchers say they've identified genetic mutations linked with a blood vessel defect that can lead to deadly brain bleeds in babies.

Gene variant found in brain complicit in MS onset

December 18, 2018
Multiple sclerosis (MS) is an autoimmune disease affecting the function of the central nervous system. Up to now, most of the 230 genetic variants associated with the disease have been linked to changes in immune cells. However, ...

Biologists identify promising drug for ALS treatment

December 18, 2018
A drug typically used to treat hepatitis could slow the progression of ALS, also known as Lou Gehrig's disease, according to new research by University of Alberta scientists.

Communication between neural networks

December 18, 2018
The brain is organized into a super-network of specialized networks of nerve cells. For such a brain architecture to function, these specialized networks – each located in a different brain area – need to be able to communicate ...

Neurons with good housekeeping are protected from Alzheimer's

December 17, 2018
Some neurons in the brain protect themselves from Alzheimer's with a cellular cleaning system that sweeps away toxic proteins associated with the disease, according to a new study from Columbia University and the University ...

0 comments

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.