The science of gambling fallacies

April 8, 2014 by Ricky Van Der Zwan, The Conversation
Black or red (or zero), the odds stay the same regardless of previous spins. Mark Seton/Flickr (cropped), Credit: CC BY-NC

Imagine yourself, a picture of sartorial elegance and sipping champagne from a crystal flute, in Le Grande Casino at Monte Carlo. It is a Monday night – in fact, the date is August 18, 1913 – and you are enjoying the tables surrounded by others, all of whom are similarly well-dressed, well-heeled and well-oiled.

You hear, from the roulette table, a growing commotion and you wander over to see what is going on. At the table you discover that the wheel has spun, 20 times in a row, black (a pattern a little less likely than tossing 20 heads in a row with a coin).

You watch with interest the next spin of the wheel. It too lands on black, as does the next spin – and the next, and the next.

With 24 spins in a row landing on black you place your chips on the table. Where would you put your money? On the black, anticipating the next spin of the wheel will land a 25th black? Or on red, reasoning that the chances of the next spin landing on black are almost zero?

If you said black, you'd have won (the wheel eventually spun 26 blacks in a row).

If you said "red" you've have exhibited a behaviour known as the "gambler's fallacy", a phenomenon which, according to a paper published today in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS), can be narrowed down to a specific part of the brain – the insular cortex.

Odds and ends

The outcome of each spin of a roulette wheel is independent of the spin before it. The ball landing on red on one spin does not affect the probability of red on the next spin, or of black. Coin tosses are the same.

In the case of French or European Roulette, de rigueur at Le Grande, the chance of spinning black, on any given , is 18/37. That means the chances of spinning 26 blacks in a row are 1 in 136,823,184, exactly the same chances of spinning 26 reds in a row, or any other 26 black/red combination.

Auntie P/Flickr, CC BY-NC-SA

However, because we have evolved in a world in which most sequential events are causally related, we have evolved processes that help us recognise those relationships. As such, most of us have difficulty in interpreting sequences of independent events as independent.

That difficulty gives rise to the gambler's fallacy: even if we know two or more events are independent it becomes difficult to ignore what just happened when trying to decide the probability of what will happen next. Instead, we develop false beliefs, or misperceptions about causal relations.

Indeed, the same processes are at work when we begin to engage in superstitious behaviours.

Introduction to the insula

The authors of today's PNAS paper have now unpacked some of the puzzle around how such misperceptions arise.

Comparing healthy participants to those with focal brain lesions, they showed that patients with damage to a brain structure called the (also known as the insula) do not suffer from false beliefs, or cognitive distortions, driven by long event sequences or near misses.

That is, their data suggest the insula plays a role in our developing beliefs about causal relations even when two events are, objectively, independent events.

The insula is an old associated with so-called homoeostatic body processes (such as taste, visceral sensations and autonomic responses like blood pressure and heart beat).

In mammals, particularly the great apes, there is evidence that the insula plays a role in the development of empathy and emotional self-awareness.

More recently, similarities between the insula in humans, some whales and dolphins and both African and Asian elephants have been reported.

All those species work with their fellows to achieve collective goals. That ability to cooperate and collaborate requires emotional control now for a potential reward later.

Both those features of the insula, autonomic processing and awareness of a person's own state, might begin to explain the role of the insula in sustaining gambling behaviours.

Gambling is exciting and a visceral activity that stimulates the autonomic nervous system as well as the dopaminergic or cortical reward systems. The paper's authors postulate that a disruption of those processes, by damage to the insula, disrupts the mechanism that reinforces gambling activity.

If they are right, they speculate, therapies that modulate insula functioning might eventually be shown also to moderate problem gambling. Perhaps they will also affect superstitious behaviours.

If so, count me out: my money would have been on 13 … black!

Explore further: Scientists identify part of brain linked to gambling addiction

Related Stories

Scientists identify part of brain linked to gambling addiction

April 7, 2014
New research reveals that brain damage affecting the insula – an area with a key role in emotions – disrupts errors of thinking linked to gambling addiction. The research, led by Dr Luke Clark from the University of Cambridge, ...

Researchers find brain's 'sweet spot' for love in neurological patient

February 14, 2014
(Medical Xpress)—A region deep inside the brain controls how quickly people make decisions about love, according to new research at the University of Chicago.

Rare neurons discovered in monkey brains

May 21, 2012
Max Planck scientists discover brain cells in monkeys that may be linked to self-awareness and empathy in humans.

Recommended for you

Cognitive training helps regain a younger-working brain

January 23, 2018
Relentless cognitive decline as we age is worrisome, and it is widely thought to be an unavoidable negative aspect of normal aging. Researchers at the Center for BrainHealth at The University of Texas at Dallas, however, ...

Lifting the veil on 'valence,' brain study reveals roots of desire, dislike

January 23, 2018
The amygdala is a tiny hub of emotions where in 2016 a team led by MIT neuroscientist Kay Tye found specific populations of neurons that assign good or bad feelings, or "valence," to experience. Learning to associate pleasure ...

Your brain responses to music reveal if you're a musician or not

January 23, 2018
How your brain responds to music listening can reveal whether you have received musical training, according to new Nordic research conducted in Finland (University of Jyväskylä and AMI Center) and Denmark (Aarhus University).

New neuron-like cells allow investigation into synthesis of vital cellular components

January 22, 2018
Neuron-like cells created from a readily available cell line have allowed researchers to investigate how the human brain makes a metabolic building block essential for the survival of all living organisms. A team led by researchers ...

Finding unravels nature of cognitive inflexibility in fragile X syndrome

January 22, 2018
Mice with the genetic defect that causes fragile X syndrome (FXS) learn and remember normally, but show an inability to learn new information that contradicts what they initially learned, shows a new study by a team of neuroscientists. ...

Epilepsy linked to brain volume and thickness differences

January 22, 2018
Epilepsy is associated with thickness and volume differences in the grey matter of several brain regions, according to new research led by UCL and the Keck School of Medicine of USC.

0 comments

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.