This article has been reviewed according to Science X's editorial process and policies. Editors have highlighted the following attributes while ensuring the content's credibility:

fact-checked

peer-reviewed publication

trusted source

proofread

Visual perception is more rational than we think, shows study

Visual perception is more rational than we think
The subjects first focus on the cross before comparing the inclination of the Gabor patches in two rounds. Credit: Schaffner et al., 2023

Are our senses there to provide us with the most complete representation of the world, or do they serve our survival? For a long time, the former was the dominant view in neuroscience. "Was" is the operative word here. In the last 50 years, psychologists such as Nobel Prize winners Daniel Kahnemann and Amos Tversky have shown that human perception is often anything but complete and instead is highly selective.

Experiments have now verified that there is a whole list of examples of cognitive biases. One of the most important is confirmation bias: we often process new information in a way that confirms our beliefs and expectations.

But up until now, researchers haven't been able to fully explain under what conditions these distortions come into play and when exactly in the perceptual process they begin. A study by a research team led by ETH Professor Rafael Polania and University of Zurich Professor Todd Hare now shows that the brain already adjusts the visual perception of things on the retina when it is in our interest to do so. Or, to put it another way, we unconsciously see things distorted when it comes to our survival, well-being or other interests. The study is published in the journal Nature Human Behaviour

How slanted are the stripe patterns?

Polania and his co-authors were able to prove through a series of experiments that people perceive the same things differently when the decision context changes. The study's 86 participants were asked to repeatedly compare two black-and-white striped patterns—known as Gabor patches—and say which pattern was closer to a 45-degree angle. The aim was to score as many points as possible.

In the first round, they received 15 points for every correct answer. But in the second round, the decision context changed: it no longer mattered whether the answer was right or wrong. Instead, the score increased continuously from 0 to 45 degrees. The participants saw the same pairs in both rounds.

The participants ought to have reached the same conclusion both times. This is because when we look at something, our retinas convert the into that is transmitted to our brain via nerve pathways. There, they are matched with our prior knowledge and experience and processed to provide a three-dimensional image. The visual information was the same in both rounds.

What we see depends on the context

When the researchers evaluated the experiment, they realized that the participants had adjusted their perceptions in the second round to score as many points as possible. If they actually saw the world objectively, there shouldn't be any differences between the two rounds.

Participants' assessments of the Gabor patches' angles ought to have been the same each time, irrespective of the decision context. But this wasn't the case. "People flexibly and unconsciously adjust their perceptions when it works to their advantage," Polania says.

For Polania and his co-authors, inferring that cognitive distortions are errors that cause us to make inaccurate or irrational judgements and decisions is missing the point. "Since our are limited, it actually makes sense that we perceive the world in a distorted or selective way," he says.

Visual perception is more rational than we think
The study participants rated the slope of the patches differently depending on whether they appeared at the bottom or top of the test field. Credit: Schaffner et.al., 2023

Even the retina prioritizes useful information

Our seems to depend more strongly on the potential utility of information than previously thought. In another experiment, the researchers were able to show that our retinas already try to process information in the most advantageous way possible.

"As soon as we look at something, we try to maximize our own benefit. This means that cognitive bias starts long before we consciously think about something," Polania says.

This is because a lot of information is lost in perception. It's therefore more efficient for the brain to filter, prioritize and select information as early on as possible.

AI filters visual information like humans

To determine when visual information is distorted, a group of participants repeated the test with a variable score. Unlike the first experiment, however, the Gabor patch pairs were displayed at the top of the visual test field. After this training round came the real task: the participants repeatedly saw a single Gabor patch at the top or bottom of the test area and had to estimate the angle of the stripes.

The researchers found that the participants assessed each patch differently depending on whether it appeared at the bottom or at the top of the test field. When subjects saw the patch at the top, their perceptions immediately adapted to the utility maximization logic they had applied during the training round. This wasn't the case when the patch appeared at the bottom.

The study's authors also tested these results on an (AI) agent that underwent the same experiments as the human subjects. To achieve the highest possible score in the experiment, the AI agent also stopped trying to represent the world accurately when it started processing the information. The agent exhibited the same perceptual biases observed in humans.

Biases are more deeply rooted than previously thought

The results of the study may also shed new light on the discussion of biases in humans and AI agents. Perhaps these distortions are so difficult to identify and change because they are an unconscious part of vision. They kick in long before we can think about what we see.

The fact that our perception is programmed to increase utility rather than to fully represent the world doesn't make things any easier. Yet, the results of the study can also help us find new ways to identify and correct biases.

More information: Jonathan Schaffner et al, Sensory perception relies on fitness-maximizing codes, Nature Human Behaviour (2023). DOI: 10.1038/s41562-023-01584-y

Journal information: Nature Human Behaviour
Provided by ETH Zurich
Citation: Visual perception is more rational than we think, shows study (2023, June 12) retrieved 27 April 2024 from https://medicalxpress.com/news/2023-06-visual-perception-rational.html
This document is subject to copyright. Apart from any fair dealing for the purpose of private study or research, no part may be reproduced without the written permission. The content is provided for information purposes only.

Explore further

'What we see seems like what we have just seen': A new study describes the potential origin of this perceptual bias

93 shares

Feedback to editors