A rose is a rose is a rose: Mathematical model explains how two brains agree on smells

May 1, 2018, Columbia University
Credit: Petr Kratochvil/public domain

In a new study, Columbia scientists have discovered why the brain's olfactory system is so remarkably consistent between individuals, even though the wiring of brain cells in this region differs greatly from person to person. To make sense of this apparent paradox, the researchers developed a computational model showing that two brains need not have previously sniffed the same exact set of odors in order to agree on a new set of scents. Instead, any two brains will know to associate new similar odors with each other (such as two different flowers) so long as both brains have experienced even the smallest overlap in odors during their lifetimes.

This work was published last week in Neuron.

"Many of the cells, or neurons, in our olfactory system are wired together seemingly at random, meaning that the neurons that activate when I smell a rose are different than yours. So why do we both agree with certainty what we're smelling?" said the paper's senior author Larry Abbott, PhD, a computational neuroscientist and principal investigator at Columbia's Mortimer B. Zuckerman Mind Brain Behavior Institute. "By creating this model, we could detect, for the first time, the patterns that underlie seemingly random activity, revealing a mathematical consistency to how our brains are identifying scents."

The journey an takes from the nose to the brain is labyrinthine. When an odor enters the nasal cavity, specialized proteins called send information about that to a designated location in the brain called the olfactory bulb. In a series of pioneering studies in the 1990s, Richard Axel, MD, a codirector at Columbia's Zuckerman Institute and a co-author of the new Neuron paper, discovered the more than 1,000 genes that encode these olfactory receptors. This work, which was performed alongside his colleague Linda B. Buck, PhD, earned them both the 2004 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine.

Today's paper focuses on how information leaves the and is interpreted by a brain region called the . The piriform cortex is believed to be a crucial structure for processing odors. Because no two whiffs of an odor are identical, the brain must make associations between odors that are similar. This process, called generalization, is what helps the brain to interpret similar smells.

"Generalization is critical because it lets you take the memory of a previous scent—such as coffee—and connect it to the odor of coffee you're currently smelling, to guide you as you stumble to the kitchen in the morning," said Evan Schaffer, PhD, a postdoctoral researcher in the Axel lab and the paper's first author.

However, as scientists have investigated the concept of generalization, they have been puzzled by two paradoxes about the piriform cortex. First, the neural activity in the piriform cortex appeared random, with no apparent logic or organization, so researchers could not tie a particular pattern of neural activity to a class of scents.

And second, the piriform cortex itself seemed too big. "Scientists could deduce a need for only about 50,000 of the roughly one-million piriform cortex neurons in the human brain," said Dr. Schaffer. "Given how energetically expensive neurons are, this raised the question: Why are there so many neurons in this part of the brain?"

The researchers developed a mathematical model that offered a resolution to both paradoxes: Two brains could indeed agree on a class of scents (i.e. fragrant flowers versus smelly garbage) if the neural activity came from a large enough pool of neurons.

The idea is similar to crowdsourcing, whereby different people each analyze one part of a complex question. That analysis is then pooled together into a central hub.

"This is analogous to what is happening in the piriform cortex," said Dr. Schaffer. "The different patterns of neural activity generated by these one-million neurons, while incomplete on their own, when combined give a complete picture of what the brain is smelling."

By then testing this model on data gathered from the brains of fruit flies, the team further showed that this neural activity helps two brains to agree on common odors, even with limited common experience.

Scientists have long argued that two brains must share a common reference point, such as each having previously smelled a rose, in order to identify the same scent. But this model suggests that the reference point can be anything—the memory of the scent of a rose can help two people agree on the smell of coffee.

"Even the tiniest bit of common experience seems to realign the brains, so that while my is different than yours, the association we each make between two related scents—such as flowers—is similar for both of us," said Dr. Schaffer.

This model, while lending insight into a long-held paradox of perception, highlights an underlying elegance to the olfactory system: despite containing different , memories and experiences—two brains can still come to an agreement.

"You and I don't need to have sniffed every type of odor in the world to come to an agreement about what we're smelling," said Dr. Schaffer. "As long we have a little bit of common experience, that's enough."

Explore further: How odours are turned into long-term memories

More information: Evan S. Schaffer et al, Odor Perception on the Two Sides of the Brain: Consistency Despite Randomness, Neuron (2018). DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.04.004

Related Stories

How odours are turned into long-term memories

December 22, 2017
The neuroscientists Dr. Christina Strauch and Prof Dr. Denise Manahan-Vaughan from the Ruhr-Universität Bochum have investigated which brain area is responsible for storing odours as long-term memories. Some odours can trigger ...

How the emotions of others influence our olfactory sense

August 24, 2017
Whether something smells good or bad, does not solely depend on one's own nose.

Study tracks what moths think when they smell with their antennae

February 27, 2018
Think of an animal tracking a scent, and you may picture a bloodhound with its nose to the ground. But a moth fluttering through the air is likely smelling for clues, too. Pollinating insects like the hawkmoth track scents ...

Understanding how neurons shape memories of smells

March 9, 2015
In a study that helps to deconstruct how olfaction is encoded in the brain, neuroscientists at University of California, San Diego School of Medicine have identified a type of neuron that appears to help tune, amplify and ...

New insights into how the human brain processes scent

June 6, 2017
Theta oscillations, a type of rhythmic electrical activity that waxes and wanes four to eight times per second, may play a fundamental role in processing scent in the human brain, according to a new study recently published ...

Ultrasound tracks odor representation in the brain

July 14, 2014
A new ultrasound imaging technique has provided the first ever in vivo visualization of activity in the piriform cortex of rats during odor perception.. This deep-seated brain structure plays an important role in olfaction, ...

Recommended for you

Research shows signalling mechanism in the brain shapes social aggression

October 19, 2018
Duke-NUS researchers have discovered that a growth factor protein, called brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), and its receptor, tropomyosin receptor kinase B (TrkB) affects social dominance in mice. The research has ...

Good spatial memory? You're likely to be good at identifying smells too

October 19, 2018
People who have better spatial memory are also better at identifying odors, according to a study published this week in Nature Communications. The study builds on a recent theory that the main reason that a sense of smell ...

How clutch molecules enable neuron migration

October 19, 2018
The brain can discriminate over 1 trillion odors. Once entering the nose, odor-related molecules activate olfactory neurons. Neuron signals first accumulate at the olfactory bulb before being passed on to activate the appropriate ...

Scientists discover the region of the brain that registers excitement over a preferred food option

October 19, 2018
At holiday buffets and potlucks, people make quick calculations about which dishes to try and how much to take of each. Johns Hopkins University neuroscientists have found a brain region that appears to be strongly connected ...

Gene plays critical role in noise-induced deafness

October 19, 2018
In experiments using mice, a team of UC San Francisco researchers has discovered a gene that plays an essential role in noise-induced deafness. Remarkably, by administering an experimental chemical—identified in a separate ...

Brain cells called astrocytes have unexpected role in brain 'plasticity'

October 18, 2018
When we're born, our brains have a great deal of flexibility. Having this flexibility to grow and change gives the immature brain the ability to adapt to new experiences and organize its interconnecting web of neural circuits. ...

0 comments

Please sign in to add a comment. Registration is free, and takes less than a minute. Read more

Click here to reset your password.
Sign in to get notified via email when new comments are made.